Different left brain regions are essential for grasping a tool compared with its subsequent use
暂无分享,去创建一个
Yong Li | Will Spijkers | Joachim Hermsdörfer | Georg Goldenberg | Jennifer Randerath | J. Hermsdörfer | Yong Li | J. Randerath | G. Goldenberg | W. Spijkers | Jennifer Randerath
[1] Eugene Tunik,et al. Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation to the Frontal Operculum and Supramarginal Gyrus Disrupts Planning of Outcome-Based Hand–Object Interactions , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[2] A. Longoni,et al. Problems in the Assessment of Hand Preference , 1985, Cortex.
[3] Leonardo Fogassi,et al. Motor functions of the parietal lobe , 2005, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.
[4] Georg Goldenberg,et al. Chapter 16 Apraxia , 2008 .
[5] Marc Jeannerod. Motor representation and control , 1990 .
[6] Hans Forssberg,et al. Phonological working memory with auditory presentation of pseudo-words — An event related fMRI Study , 2008, Brain Research.
[7] S. Frey. Tool use, communicative gesture and cerebral asymmetries in the modern human brain , 2008, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[8] M. Goodale,et al. Two visual systems re-viewed , 2008, Neuropsychologia.
[9] D. Rosenbaum,et al. Planning for manual positioning: the end-state comfort effect for manual abduction–adduction , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.
[10] R. E Passingham,et al. Activations related to “mirror” and “canonical” neurones in the human brain: an fMRI study , 2003, NeuroImage.
[11] J Hermsdörfer,et al. The effect of tactile feedback on pantomime of tool use in apraxia , 2004, Neurology.
[12] Scott T Grafton,et al. Repetition suppression for performed hand gestures revealed by fMRI , 2009, Human brain mapping.
[13] B. Mesquita,et al. Adjustment to Chronic Diseases and Terminal Illness Health Psychology : Psychological Adjustment to Chronic Disease , 2006 .
[14] Karalyn Patterson,et al. Apraxia, mechanical problem solving and semantic knowledge: contributions to object usage in corticobasal degeneration. , 2002, Journal of neurology.
[15] J. Jonides,et al. Storage and executive processes in the frontal lobes. , 1999, Science.
[16] Georg Goldenberg,et al. Defective pantomime of object use in left brain damage: apraxia or asymbolia? , 2003, Neuropsychologia.
[17] JamesW. Lewis. Cortical Networks Related to Human Use of Tools , 2006, The Neuroscientist : a review journal bringing neurobiology, neurology and psychiatry.
[18] C. Rorden,et al. Stereotaxic display of brain lesions. , 2000, Behavioural neurology.
[19] Gereon R. Fink,et al. Functional Interactions during the Retrieval of Conceptual Action Knowledge: An fMRI Study , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[20] Joseph S. Gati,et al. Visually-guided grasping produces fMRI activation in dorsal but not ventral stream brain areas , 2010 .
[21] M. Gentilucci,et al. Planning for action , 2020, Coaching with Research in Mind.
[22] Georg Goldenberg,et al. The Neural Basis of Imitation is Body Part Specific , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[23] Scott T Grafton,et al. Differential Recruitment of Anterior Intraparietal Sulcus and Superior Parietal Lobule during Visually Guided Grasping Revealed by Electrical Neuroimaging , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[24] Matthew F. S. Rushworth,et al. Parietal rTMS Disrupts the Initiation but not the Execution of On-line Adjustments to a Perturbation of Object Size , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[25] A. Murata,et al. Cortical connections of the macaque anterior intraparietal (AIP) area. , 2008, Cerebral cortex.
[26] Chris Rorden,et al. Lesion Mapping of Cognitive Abilities Linked to Intelligence , 2009, Neuron.
[27] Afdc Hamilton,et al. The motor hierarchy: from kinematics to goals and intentions , 2007 .
[28] L. Buxbaum,et al. Knowledge of object manipulation and object function: dissociations in apraxic and nonapraxic subjects , 2002, Brain and Language.
[29] M. Arbib,et al. Language within our grasp , 1998, Trends in Neurosciences.
[30] Sarah H. Creem,et al. Grasping objects by their handles: a necessary interaction between cognition and action. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[31] J. Hermsdörfer,et al. Grasping tools: Effects of task and apraxia , 2009, Neuropsychologia.
[32] Scott T. Grafton,et al. Virtual lesions of the anterior intraparietal area disrupt goal-dependent on-line adjustments of grasp , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.
[33] G. Goldenberg,et al. The neural basis of tool use. , 2009, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[34] S. Frey. What Puts the How in Where? Tool Use and the Divided Visual Streams Hypothesis , 2007, Cortex.
[35] Eric Achten,et al. Tool responsive regions in the posterior parietal cortex: Effect of differences in motor goal and target object during imagined transitive movements , 2009, NeuroImage.
[36] G. Rizzolatti,et al. Two different streams form the dorsal visual system: anatomy and functions , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.
[37] Frank Marchak,et al. Constraints for Action Selection: Overhand Versus Underhand Grips , 2018, Attention and Performance XIII.
[38] Scott T. Grafton,et al. Premotor Cortex Activation during Observation and Naming of Familiar Tools , 1997, NeuroImage.
[39] L. Buxbaum,et al. Action knowledge, visuomotor activation, and embodiment in the two action systems , 2010, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.
[40] M. Jeannerod. The timing of natural prehension movements. , 1984, Journal of motor behavior.
[41] Ravi S. Menon,et al. Visually guided grasping produces fMRI activation in dorsal but not ventral stream brain areas , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.
[42] Alex Martin,et al. A neural system for learning about object function. , 2006, Cerebral cortex.
[43] Thomas E. Nichols,et al. Rank-order versus mean based statistics for neuroimaging , 2007, NeuroImage.
[44] Alex Martin,et al. Grounding Object Concepts in Perception and Action: Evidence from FMRI Studies of Tools , 2007, Cortex.
[45] Edward E. Smith,et al. The Neural Basis for Categorization in Semantic Memory , 2002, NeuroImage.
[46] M. Short,et al. Planning macroscopic aspects of manual control: end-state comfort and point-of-change effects. , 1997, Acta psychologica.
[47] G. Goldenberg,et al. Tool use and mechanical problem solving in apraxia , 1998, Neuropsychologia.
[48] J. Hermsdörfer,et al. Neural representations of pantomimed and actual tool use: Evidence from an event-related fMRI study , 2007, NeuroImage.
[49] G. Rizzolatti,et al. Parietal Lobe: From Action Organization to Intention Understanding , 2005, Science.
[50] M. Jeannerod,et al. Constraints on human arm movement trajectories. , 1987, Canadian journal of psychology.
[51] Murray Grossman,et al. Left Inferior Parietal Representations for Skilled Hand-Object Interactions: Evidence from Stroke and Corticobasal Degeneration , 2007, Cortex.
[52] Wolfgang Weinrich,et al. Klinische Neuroanatomie und kranielle Bilddiagnostik , 2007 .
[53] James N. Lee,et al. The influence of complex action knowledge on representations of novel graspable objects: Evidence from functional magnetic resonance imaging , 2007, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.
[54] M. Goodale,et al. Separate visual pathways for perception and action , 1992, Trends in Neurosciences.
[55] M. L. Lambon Ralph,et al. The role of conceptual knowledge in object use evidence from semantic dementia. , 2000, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[56] R L Klatzky,et al. Planning for hand shape and arm transport when reaching for objects. , 1995, Acta psychologica.
[57] Alessia Tessari,et al. Neuropsychological evidence for a strategic control of multiple routes in imitation. , 2006, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[58] C. Armbrüster,et al. Movement planning in prehension: do intended actions influence the initial reach and grasp movement? , 2006, Motor control.