Substance and bias in social desirability responding

In this study, self-reports from N=67 participants were compared to the external criterion of three observer ratings on the Big Five personality traits. In addition, Self-Deceptive Enhancement (SDE) and Impression Management (IM) were assessed with a shortened version of the Balanced Inventory of Desirable Responding (BIDR-6; Paulhus, 1991) in German language. Hypotheses were derived from the model of Paulhus and John (1998), who argued for the existence of two self-favouring tendencies: egoistic and moralistic bias. Firstly, we calculated self-report inflation or bias scores by regressing self-reports on observer ratings. Residual scores of this analysis were correlated with SDE and IM. In accordance with our expectations, SDE was positively correlated with bias scores of emotional stability, extraversion, and openness, whereas IM was positively related to bias scores of agreeableness and conscientiousness. Secondly, self-observer agreement was unaffected or even decreased when self-reports were corrected for SDE and IM. Results were discussed with regard to their implications for further research in socially desirable responding.

[1]  R. Davidson,et al.  Consciousness and Self-Regulation: Advances in Research and Theory IV , 1976 .

[2]  R. McCrae,et al.  On the invalidity of validity scales: evidence from self-reports and observer ratings in volunteer samples. , 2000, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[3]  B. Fritzsche,et al.  The usefulness of the NEO PI-R positive presentation management scale for detecting response distortion in employment contexts , 2001 .

[4]  John P. Robinson,et al.  Measures Of Personality And Social Psychological Attitudes , 1991 .

[5]  O. John,et al.  Accuracy and bias in self-perception: individual differences in self-enhancement and the role of narcissism. , 1994, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[6]  P. Costa,et al.  Stability and change in personality assessment: the revised NEO Personality Inventory in the year 2000. , 1997, Journal of personality assessment.

[7]  John P. Robinson,et al.  Measures of social psychological attitudes , 1976 .

[8]  D. Paulhus Two-component models of socially desirable responding. , 1984 .

[9]  D. Paulhus,et al.  Socially desirable responding and sexuality self‐reports , 1998 .

[10]  Ronald R. Holden,et al.  Comparisons among the Holden Psychological Screening Inventory (HPSI), the Brief Symptom Inventory (BSI), and the Balanced Inventory of Desirable Responding (BIDR) , 2000, Assessment.

[11]  D. Paulhus Measurement and control of response bias. , 1991 .

[12]  G. Stokes,et al.  Content/Construct Approaches in Life History Form Development for Selection , 2001 .

[13]  Ruben C. Gur,et al.  Self-Deception, Self-Confrontation, and Consciousness , 1978 .

[14]  Chockalingam Viswesvaran,et al.  Role of social desirability in personality testing for personnel selection: The red herring. , 1996 .

[15]  Paul T. Costa,et al.  Social desirability scales: More substance than style. , 1983 .

[16]  Jack Block,et al.  The challenge of response sets , 1965 .

[17]  Delroy L. Paulhus,et al.  Enhancement and Denial in Socially Desirable Responding , 1991 .

[18]  Murray R. Barrick,et al.  Effects of impression management and self-deception on the predictive validity of personality constructs. , 1996, The Journal of applied psychology.

[19]  J. S. Wiggins,et al.  Convergences Among Stylistic Response Measures from Objective Personality Tests , 1964 .

[20]  Delroy L. Paulhus,et al.  Egoistic and Moralistic Biases in Self-Perception: The Interplay of Self-Deceptive Styles With Basic Traits and Motives , 1998 .

[21]  P. Borkenau,et al.  Social desirability scales as moderator and suppressor variables , 1992 .