Characterizing the Musical Phenotype in Individuals With Williams Syndrome

Williams Syndrome (WS), a neurodevelopmental genetic disorder, is characterized by peaks and valleys in mental function: substantial impairments in cognitive domains such as reasoning, arithmetic ability, and spatial cognition, alongside relatively preserved skills in social domains, face processing, language, and music. We report the results of a comprehensive survey on musical behaviors and background administered to the largest sample of individuals with WS to date (n = 118, mean age = 20.4), and compare the results to those obtained from a control group of typically developing normal individuals (n = 118, mean age = 20.9) and two groups of individuals with other neurodevelopmental genetic disorders, Autism (n = 30, mean age = 18.2) and Down Syndrome (n = 40, mean age = 17.2). Individuals with WS were found to be rated higher in musical accomplishment, engagement, and interest than either of the comparison groups, and equivalent on most measures to the control group. Compared to all other groups including the controls, the WS individuals displayed greater emotional responses to music, manifested interest in music at an earlier age, and spent more hours per week listening to music. In addition, the effects of music listening (whether positive or negative) tended to last longer in the WS group. A factor analysis extracted seven principal components that characterize the musical phenotype in our sample, and discriminant function analysis of those factors was able to successfully predict group membership for the majority of cases. We discuss the neurobiological implications of these findings.

[1]  W. F. Vonderhaar A COMPARATIVE STUDY OF PERFORMANCE SCALE IQ'S AND SUBTEST SCORES OF DEAF CHILDREN ON THE WECHSLER INTELLIGENCE SCALE FOR CHILDREN AND THE WECHSLER INTELLIGENCE SCALE FOR CHILDREN-REVISED , 1977 .

[2]  Linda Pring,et al.  Autism and pitch processing: A precursor for savant musical ability. , 1998 .

[3]  Ursula Bellugi,et al.  Bridging cognition, the brain and molecular genetics: evidence from Williams syndrome , 1999, Trends in Neurosciences.

[4]  J. R. Landis,et al.  The measurement of observer agreement for categorical data. , 1977, Biometrics.

[5]  U. Francke Williams-Beuren syndrome: genes and mechanisms. , 1999, Human molecular genetics.

[6]  S. Baron-Cohen,et al.  The "Reading the Mind in the Eyes" Test revised version: a study with normal adults, and adults with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. , 2001, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[7]  B. Hermelin,et al.  A pseudo-savant: A case of exceptional musical splinter skills , 1999 .

[8]  C. Cytrynbaum,et al.  Music Skills and the Expressive Interpretation of Music in Children with Williams-Beuren Syndrome: Pitch, Rhythm, Melodic Imagery, Phrasing, and Musical Affect , 2001, Child neuropsychology : a journal on normal and abnormal development in childhood and adolescence.

[9]  O. Udwin,et al.  A SURVEY OF ADULTS WITH WILLIAMS SYNDROME AND IDIOPATHIC INFANTILE HYPERCALCAEMIA , 1990, Developmental medicine and child neurology.

[10]  S. Baron-Cohen,et al.  Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind” ? , 1985, Cognition.

[11]  B. Hermelin,et al.  Can children with autistic spectrum disorders perceive affect in music? An experimental investigation , 1999, Psychological Medicine.

[12]  K. Sullivan,et al.  A componential view of theory of mind: evidence from Williams syndrome , 2000, Cognition.

[13]  J. Aggleton,et al.  Handedness and Musical Ability: A Study of Professional Orchestral Players, Composers, and Choir Members , 1994 .

[14]  C. Kasari,et al.  Emotion recognition by children with Down syndrome. , 2001, American journal of mental retardation : AJMR.

[15]  Ursula Bellugi,et al.  V. Multi-Level Analysis of Cortical Neuroanatomy in Williams Syndrome , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[16]  Waldo Emerson Nelson,et al.  Textbook of Pediatrics , 1969 .

[17]  U. Bellugi,et al.  Narrative as a social engagement tool: the excessive use of evaluation in narratives from children with williams syndrome , 2000 .

[18]  S. Luthar,et al.  Developmental psychopathology : perspectives on adjustment, risk, and disorder , 1997 .

[19]  Rp Hobson,et al.  Components of person perception: An investigation with autistic, non‐autistic retarded and typically developing children and adolescents , 1997 .

[20]  I. Rapin Handbook of neurodevelopmental and genetic disorders in children , 2000 .

[21]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  The Organization of Brain Activations in Number Comparison: Event-Related Potentials and the Additive-Factors Method , 1996, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[22]  J. Elman,et al.  Rethinking Innateness: A Connectionist Perspective on Development , 1996 .

[23]  U Bellugi,et al.  Williams syndrome and the brain. , 1997, Scientific American.

[24]  I. Mcmanus,et al.  HANDEDNESS IN THE MENTALLY HANDICAPPED , 1985, Developmental medicine and child neurology.

[25]  R. Koegel,et al.  Measuring musical abilities of autistic children , 1979, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[26]  B. Rourke,et al.  Music and Language Skills of Children with Williams Syndrome , 1999 .

[27]  Stephan Eliez,et al.  IV. Neuroanatomy of Williams Syndrome: A High-Resolution MRI Study , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[28]  Susan B. Gall,et al.  The Gale Encyclopedia of Childhood and Adolescence. , 1998 .

[29]  R. Hagerman Neurodevelopmental Disorders: Diagnosis and Treatment , 1999 .

[30]  I. Peretz,et al.  Local and global processing of music in high-functioning persons with autism: beyond central coherence? , 2000, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[31]  Vinod Menon,et al.  Musical structure is processed in “language” areas of the brain: a possible role for Brodmann Area 47 in temporal coherence , 2003, NeuroImage.

[32]  R. Adolphs,et al.  II. Hypersociability in Williams Syndrome , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[33]  M. Coltheart,et al.  Modularity of music processing , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.

[34]  E. Marg A VISION OF THE BRAIN , 1994 .

[35]  G. Shuttleworth Mentally Deficient Children: their Treatment and Training , 2007, Nature.

[36]  J. Denie,et al.  Supravalvular Aortic Stenosis , 1958, Circulation.

[37]  Mark Noble,et al.  LIM-kinase1 Hemizygosity Implicated in Impaired Visuospatial Constructive Cognition , 1996, Cell.

[38]  D. Cicchetti,et al.  Talking about self and other: Emergence of an internal state lexicon in young children with Down syndrome , 1997, Development and Psychopathology.

[39]  Gary H. Glover,et al.  Neural Correlates of Auditory Perception in Williams Syndrome: An fMRI Study , 2003, NeuroImage.

[40]  John A. Sloboda,et al.  Music: Where Cognition and Emotion Meet , 1999 .

[41]  M. Butler,et al.  Photoanthropometric study of craniofacial traits in individuals with Williams syndrome , 1997, Clinical genetics.

[42]  U. Bellugi,et al.  Williams syndrome: an exploration of neurocognitive and genetic features , 2001, Clinical Neuroscience Research.

[43]  Ursula Bellugi,et al.  Aversion, awareness, and attraction: investigating claims of hyperacusis in the Williams syndrome phenotype. , 2005, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[44]  Aniruddh D. Patel,et al.  Language, music, syntax and the brain , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.

[45]  Ursula Bellugi,et al.  I. The Neurocognitive Profile of Williams Syndrome: A Complex Pattern of Strengths and Weaknesses , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[46]  U. Bellugi,et al.  Musical Abilities in Individuals with Williams Syndrome , 1998 .

[47]  I. Cross Music, Cognition, Culture, and Evolution , 2001, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[48]  U. Bellugi,et al.  Music cognition and Williams Syndrome , 1999 .

[49]  P. Heaton,et al.  Pitch memory, labelling and disembedding in autism. , 2003, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[50]  G. Johnson,et al.  Nature Encyclopedia of the Human Genome , 2004 .

[51]  M. Hassler,et al.  Functional brain organization, handedness, and immune vulnerability in musicians and non-musicians , 1993, Neuropsychologia.

[52]  B. Stratford,et al.  Responses to music and movement in the development of children with Down's syndrome. , 2008, Journal of mental deficiency research.

[53]  Carolyn B. Mervis,et al.  Williams syndrome: Findings from an integrated program of research , 1999 .

[54]  Virginia Volterra,et al.  Linguistic Abilities in Italian Children With Williams Syndrome , 1996, Cortex.

[55]  D. Huron,et al.  Is Music an Evolutionary Adaptation? , 2001, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[56]  S. Coren,et al.  Lateral preferences and human behavior , 1981 .

[57]  R. Zatorre Absolute pitch: a model for understanding the influence of genes and development on neural and cognitive function , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.

[58]  A. Karmiloff-Smith Development itself is the key to understanding developmental disorders , 1998, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[59]  C. Donnelly Handbook of Neurodevelopmental and Genetic Disorders in Children , 2002 .

[60]  Gregory A. Bryant,et al.  Music and dance as a coalition signaling system , 2003, Human nature.

[61]  Ursula Bellugi,et al.  Face and place processing in Williams syndrome: evidence for a dorsal-ventral dissociation , 2002, Neuroreport.

[62]  A. Gosch,et al.  Personality characteristics and behaviour problems in individuals of different ages with Williams syndrome , 1997, Developmental medicine and child neurology.

[63]  A. J. Klein,et al.  Hyperacusis and otitis media in individuals with Williams syndrome. , 1990, The Journal of speech and hearing disorders.

[64]  T. Sejnowski,et al.  Irresistible environment meets immovable neurons , 1997, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[65]  P. McCarthy,et al.  Validity studies on the Illinois test of psycholinguistic abilities , 1964 .

[66]  Cindy Lu Edgerton The Effect of Improvisational Music Therapy on the Communicative Behaviors of Autistic Children , 1994 .

[67]  Allan L. Reiss,et al.  Williams Syndrome: A Neurogenetic Model of Human Behavior , 2006 .

[68]  Helen J. Neville,et al.  Neurophysiological markers of face processing in Williams syndrome. , 2001 .

[69]  A. C. Sherwin Reactions to music of autistic (schizophrenic) children. , 1953, The American journal of psychiatry.

[70]  F. Previc Nonright‐handedness, central nervous system and related pathology, and its lateralization: A reformulation and synthesis , 1996 .

[71]  W. Yule,et al.  A cognitive and behavioural phenotype in Williams syndrome. , 1991, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[72]  A. Beuren,et al.  THE SYNDROME OF SUPRAVALVULAR AORTIC STENOSIS, PERIPHERAL PULMONARY STENOSIS, MENTAL RETARDATION AND SIMILAR FACIAL APPEARANCE. , 1964, The American journal of cardiology.

[73]  V. Volterra,et al.  Children with Williams syndrome : Is there a single neuropsychological profile? , 1999 .

[74]  Terry L. Jernigan,et al.  Language, cognition, and brain organization in a neurodevelopmental disorder. , 1992 .

[75]  R. Adolphs,et al.  Towards the neural basis for hypersociability in a genetic syndrome. , 1999, Neuroreport.

[76]  Justine Sergent,et al.  Mapping the musician brain , 1993 .

[77]  C. Bormann-Kischkel,et al.  The development of emotional concepts in autism. , 1995, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[78]  O. S. Marin,et al.  Neurological Aspects of Music Perception and Performance , 1999 .

[79]  Ursula Bellugi,et al.  “Everybody in the world is my friend” hypersociability in young children with Williams syndrome , 2004, American journal of medical genetics. Part A.

[80]  A. F. Tredgold Mental Deficiency (Amentia) , 1929, The Indian Medical Gazette.

[81]  Rumiko Matsuoka,et al.  VI. Genome Structure and Cognitive Map of Williams Syndrome , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[82]  Ursula Bellugi,et al.  Journey from Cognition to Brain to Gene: Perspectives from Williams Syndrome , 2001 .

[83]  F. Greenberg,et al.  Williams syndrome. , 1988, Pediatrics.