It takes two to tango: Suppression of task-irrelevant features requires (spatial) competition
暂无分享,去创建一个
Matthias M. Müller | Norman Forschack | Berit Brummerloh | Christopher Gundlach | Norman Forschack | C. Gundlach | B. Brummerloh
[1] Matthias M. Müller,et al. Attentional Selection of Feature Conjunctions Is Accomplished by Parallel and Independent Selection of Single Features , 2015, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[2] R. Desimone,et al. Neural mechanisms of spatial selective attention in areas V1, V2, and V4 of macaque visual cortex. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.
[3] Matthias M. Müller,et al. Effects of spatial selective attention on the steady-state visual evoked potential in the 20-28 Hz range. , 1998, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.
[4] S. A. Hillyard,et al. Sustained division of the attentional spotlight , 2003, Nature.
[5] Mark W. Becker,et al. No templates for rejection: a failure to configure attention to ignore task-irrelevant features , 2015 .
[6] A. Treisman,et al. A feature-integration theory of attention , 1980, Cognitive Psychology.
[7] Matthias M. Müller. Location and features of instructive spatial cues do not influence the time course of covert shifts of visual spatial attention , 2008, Biological Psychology.
[8] Stefan Treue,et al. Feature-based attention influences motion processing gain in macaque visual cortex , 1999, Nature.
[9] S. Treue. Neural correlates of attention in primate visual cortex , 2001, Trends in Neurosciences.
[10] R. M. Boynton,et al. Comparison of four methods of heterochromatic photometry. , 1972, Journal of the Optical Society of America.
[11] Daniël Lakens,et al. Calculating and reporting effect sizes to facilitate cumulative science: a practical primer for t-tests and ANOVAs , 2013, Front. Psychol..
[12] S. Hillyard,et al. Event-related brain potentials in the study of visual selective attention. , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[13] Dennis Gabor,et al. Theory of communication , 1946 .
[14] Joy J Geng,et al. Distractor Probability Changes the Shape of the Attentional Template , 2017, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[15] Joshua B. Ewen,et al. Inhibition Drives Early Feature-Based Attention , 2014, Psychological science.
[16] Arnaud Delorme,et al. EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis , 2004, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.
[17] Susan L. Travis,et al. Neural Responses to Target Features outside a Search Array Are Enhanced during Conjunction but Not Unique-Feature Search , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[18] Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al. The neural basis of biased competition in human visual cortex , 2001, Neuropsychologia.
[19] Ben M. Crittenden,et al. Distinct Mechanisms for Distractor Suppression and Target Facilitation , 2016, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[20] Howard E Egeth,et al. The ignoring paradox: Cueing distractor features leads first to selection, then to inhibition of to-be-ignored items , 2012, Attention, perception & psychophysics.
[21] S. Yantis. Control of visual attention. , 1998 .
[22] Steven J. Luck,et al. The Role of Inhibition in Avoiding Distraction by Salient Stimuli , 2018, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[23] D. Regan. Human brain electrophysiology: Evoked potentials and evoked magnetic fields in science and medicine , 1989 .
[24] Matthias M. Müller,et al. Independent effects of attentional gain control and competitive interactions on visual stimulus processing. , 2013, Cerebral cortex.
[25] S. Andersen,et al. Behavioral performance follows the time course of neural facilitation and suppression during cued shifts of feature-selective attention , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[26] Jason T. Arita,et al. Templates for rejection: configuring attention to ignore task-irrelevant features. , 2012, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[27] Søren K. Andersen,et al. Global Enhancement but Local Suppression in Feature-based Attention , 2016, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[28] Matthias M. Müller,et al. Concurrent recording of steady-state and transient event-related potentials as indices of visual-spatial selective attention , 2000, Clinical Neurophysiology.
[29] G. Boynton,et al. Global effects of feature-based attention in human visual cortex , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.
[30] Jürgen Kayser,et al. Principal components analysis of Laplacian waveforms as a generic method for identifying ERP generator patterns: I. Evaluation with auditory oddball tasks , 2006, Clinical Neurophysiology.
[31] S. Luck,et al. Feature-based attention modulates feedforward visual processing , 2009, Nature Neuroscience.
[32] D. Heeger,et al. The Normalization Model of Attention , 2009, Neuron.
[33] Joonyeol Lee,et al. A Normalization Model of Attentional Modulation of Single Unit Responses , 2009, PloS one.
[34] Andrew Hollingworth,et al. Evidence for negative feature guidance in visual search is explained by spatial recoding. , 2015, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[35] Jasna Martinovic,et al. Bottom-Up Biases in Feature-Selective Attention , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[36] Hermann J. Müller,et al. Attentional capture in visual search: Capture and post-capture dynamics revealed by EEG , 2017, NeuroImage.
[37] Søren K. Andersen,et al. Effects of Feature-selective and Spatial Attention at Different Stages of Visual Processing , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[38] Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al. Modulation of sensory suppression: implications for receptive field sizes in the human visual cortex. , 2001, Journal of neurophysiology.
[39] B. Rockstroh,et al. Statistical control of artifacts in dense array EEG/MEG studies. , 2000, Psychophysiology.
[40] R. Desimone,et al. Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. , 1995, Annual review of neuroscience.
[41] S A Hillyard,et al. Feature-selective attention enhances color signals in early visual areas of the human brain , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[42] Carly J. Leonard,et al. Direct Evidence for Active Suppression of Salient-but-Irrelevant Sensory Inputs , 2015, Psychological science.
[43] J Duncan,et al. Responses of neurons in macaque area V4 during memory-guided visual search. , 2001, Cerebral cortex.
[44] Steven A. Hillyard,et al. Global Facilitation of Attended Features Is Obligatory and Restricts Divided Attention , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[45] R. Desimone. Visual attention mediated by biased competition in extrastriate visual cortex. , 1998, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.
[46] J. Wolfe,et al. Guided Search 2.0 A revised model of visual search , 1994, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[47] Steven J. Luck,et al. Suppression of overt attentional capture by salient-but-irrelevant color singletons , 2016, Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics.
[48] Steven A. Hillyard,et al. Attention Facilitates Multiple Stimulus Features in Parallel in Human Visual Cortex , 2008, Current Biology.
[49] Matthias M. Müller,et al. The time course of cortical facilitation during cued shifts of spatial attention , 1998, Nature Neuroscience.
[50] Matthias M. Müller,et al. Color-selective attention need not be mediated by spatial attention. , 2009, Journal of vision.
[51] T. Ferrée,et al. Spherical Splines and Average Referencing in Scalp Electroencephalography , 2006, Brain Topography.
[52] John M. Gaspar,et al. Suppression of Salient Objects Prevents Distraction in Visual Search , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[53] Viola S. Störmer,et al. Feature-Based Attention Elicits Surround Suppression in Feature Space , 2014, Current Biology.