Failure to disrupt the ‘sensorimotor’ memory for lifting objects with a precision grip

When repetitively lifting an object with mechanical properties that vary from lift-to-lift, the fingertip forces for gripping and lifting are influenced strongly by the previous lift, revealing a ‘sensorimotor’ memory. Two recent reports indicate that the sensorimotor memory for grip force is easily disrupted by an unrelated task like a strong pinch or vibration, even when the lift was performed with the hand contralateral to the vibration or preceding pinch. These findings indicate that this memory may reflect sensory input or muscle contraction levels, rather than object properties or the specific task of gripping and lifting. Here we report that the predictive scaling of lift force was not disrupted by conditioning tasks that featured exerting a vertical isometric force with the upper extremity. When subjects lifted a 2 N object repetitively the peak lift force rate was 26.4 N/s. The lift force rate increased to 36.1 N/s when the 2 N object was lifted (regardless of hand) after lifting the 8 N object with the right hand, which reveals the expected ‘sensorimotor’ memory. The lift force rate did not increase (24.8 vs. 26.4 N/s for the control condition) when a bout of isometric exertion (9.8 N) in the vertical direction with the distal right forearm preceded lifts of the 2 N object. This finding was confirmed with another isometric task designed to more closely mimic lifting an object with a precision grip. This difference in the sensitivity of grip versus lift force to a preceding isometric contraction indicates that separate sensorimotor memories contribute to the predictive scaling of the commands for gripping and lifting an object.

[1]  T Brochier,et al.  Neuronal activity in somatosensory cortex of monkeys using a precision grip. II. Responses To object texture and weights. , 1999, Journal of neurophysiology.

[2]  Joachim Hermsdörfer,et al.  Memory for fingertip forces: passive hand muscle vibration interferes with predictive grip force scaling , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[3]  R. Johansson,et al.  Development of human precision grip , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[4]  P Jenmalm,et al.  Visual and Somatosensory Information about Object Shape Control Manipulative Fingertip Forces , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[5]  Hiroshi Kinoshita,et al.  Functional brain areas used for the lifting of objects using a precision grip: a PET study , 2000, Brain Research.

[6]  T Brochier,et al.  Activity in ventral and dorsal premotor cortex in response to predictable force-pulse perturbations in a precision grip task. , 2001, Journal of neurophysiology.

[7]  P. R. Davidson,et al.  Widespread access to predictive models in the motor system: a short review , 2005, Journal of neural engineering.

[8]  R. Johansson,et al.  Development of human precision grip I: Basic coordination of force , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[9]  H. Forssberg,et al.  Lighter or Heavier Than Predicted: Neural Correlates of Corrective Mechanisms during Erroneously Programmed Lifts , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[10]  R. Johansson,et al.  Evidence for the involvement of the posterior parietal cortex in coordination of fingertip forces for grasp stability in manipulation. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[11]  R. Johansson,et al.  Integration of sensory information during the programming of precision grip: comments on the contributions of size cues , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[12]  T Brochier,et al.  Neuronal activity in somatosensory cortex of monkeys using a precision grip. III. Responses to altered friction perturbations. , 1999, Journal of neurophysiology.

[13]  H. Forssberg,et al.  Differential fronto-parietal activation depending on force used in a precision grip task: an fMRI study. , 2001, Journal of neurophysiology.

[14]  R. S. Johansson,et al.  Roles of glabrous skin receptors and sensorimotor memory in automatic control of precision grip when lifting rougher or more slippery objects , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[15]  R. Johansson,et al.  Cortical activity in precision- versus power-grip tasks: an fMRI study. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[16]  A. M. Smith,et al.  Responses of cerebellar Purkinje cells to slip of a hand-held object. , 1992, Journal of neurophysiology.

[17]  K. J. Cole,et al.  Sensory-motor coordination during grasping and manipulative actions , 1992, Current Biology.

[18]  T Brochier,et al.  Neuronal activity in somatosensory cortex of monkeys using a precision grip. I. Receptive fields and discharge patterns. , 1999, Journal of neurophysiology.

[19]  P Jenmalm,et al.  Visual and tactile information about object-curvature control fingertip forces and grasp kinematics in human dexterous manipulation. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[20]  Carolyn R. Mason,et al.  Purkinje cells signal hand shape and grasp force during reach-to-grasp in the monkey. , 2006, Journal of neurophysiology.

[21]  Hans Forssberg,et al.  Human brain activity in the control of fine static precision grip forces: an fMRI study , 2001, The European journal of neuroscience.

[22]  Hans Forssberg,et al.  Brain activity during predictable and unpredictable weight changes when lifting objects. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[23]  P. Gribble,et al.  Are there distinct neural representations of object and limb dynamics? , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.

[24]  R. Johansson,et al.  Visual size cues in the programming of manipulative forces during precision grip , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[25]  R. Johansson,et al.  Coordinated isometric muscle commands adequately and erroneously programmed for the weight during lifting task with precision grip , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[26]  J R Flanagan,et al.  The Role of Internal Models in Motion Planning and Control: Evidence from Grip Force Adjustments during Movements of Hand-Held Loads , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[27]  A. M. Smith,et al.  Purkinje cell simple spike activity during grasping and lifting objects of different textures and weights. , 1990, Journal of neurophysiology.

[28]  K. J. Cole,et al.  Memory representations underlying motor commands used during manipulation of common and novel objects. , 1993, Journal of neurophysiology.

[29]  Joël Monzée,et al.  Responses of cerebellar interpositus neurons to predictable perturbations applied to an object held in a precision grip. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.

[30]  K. J. Cole,et al.  Sensorimotor Memory For Fingertip Forces: Evidence For A Task-Independent Motor Memory , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[31]  K. J. Cole,et al.  Old age impairs the use of arbitrary visual cues for predictive control of fingertip forces during grasp , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.

[32]  Hans Forssberg,et al.  Formation and lateralization of internal representations underlying motor commands during precision grip , 1994, Neuropsychologia.