Our Faces in the Dog's Brain: Functional Imaging Reveals Temporal Cortex Activation during Perception of Human Faces

Dogs have a rich social relationship with humans. One fundamental aspect of it is how dogs pay close attention to human faces in order to guide their behavior, for example, by recognizing their owner and his/her emotional state using visual cues. It is well known that humans have specific brain regions for the processing of other human faces, yet it is unclear how dogs’ brains process human faces. For this reason, our study focuses on describing the brain correlates of perception of human faces in dogs using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). We trained seven domestic dogs to remain awake, still and unrestrained inside an MRI scanner. We used a visual stimulation paradigm with block design to compare activity elicited by human faces against everyday objects. Brain activity related to the perception of faces changed significantly in several brain regions, but mainly in the bilateral temporal cortex. The opposite contrast (i.e., everyday objects against human faces) showed no significant brain activity change. The temporal cortex is part of the ventral visual pathway, and our results are consistent with reports in other species like primates and sheep, that suggest a high degree of evolutionary conservation of this pathway for face processing. This study introduces the temporal cortex as candidate to process human faces, a pillar of social cognition in dogs.

[1]  J. Fuster Prefrontal Cortex , 2018 .

[2]  N. Rooney,et al.  Ádám Miklósi Dog Behaviour, Evolution, and Cognition , 2008, Animal Behaviour.

[3]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Making inferences about the location of hidden food: social dog, causal ape. , 2006, Journal of comparative psychology.

[4]  N. Kanwisher Functional specificity in the human brain: A window into the functional architecture of the mind , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[5]  Heini Törnqvist,et al.  Dogs do look at images: eye tracking in canine cognition research , 2012, Animal Cognition.

[6]  K. Kendrick,et al.  Behavioural and neurophysiological evidence for face identity and face emotion processing in animals , 2006, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[7]  G. Vallortigara,et al.  Sniffing with the right nostril: lateralization of response to odour stimuli by dogs , 2011, Animal Behaviour.

[8]  L. Parr,et al.  The evolution of face processing in primates , 2011, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[9]  N. Kanwisher,et al.  The Fusiform Face Area: A Module in Human Extrastriate Cortex Specialized for Face Perception , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[10]  Doris Y. Tsao,et al.  Functional Compartmentalization and Viewpoint Generalization Within the Macaque Face-Processing System , 2010, Science.

[11]  J. Haxby,et al.  The distributed human neural system for face perception , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[12]  A. Martínez,et al.  The AR face databasae , 1998 .

[13]  Jonathan W. Peirce,et al.  PsychoPy—Psychophysics software in Python , 2007, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[14]  Ludwig Huber,et al.  Discrimination of familiar human faces in dogs (Canis familiaris) , 2013, Learning and motivation.

[15]  L. Parr,et al.  Face Processing in the Chimpanzee Brain , 2009, Current Biology.

[16]  Á. Miklósi,et al.  A Simple Reason for a Big Difference Wolves Do Not Look Back at Humans, but Dogs Do , 2003, Current Biology.

[17]  Doris Y. Tsao,et al.  Comparing face patch systems in macaques and humans , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[18]  Gregory S. Berns,et al.  Scent of the familiar: An fMRI study of canine brain responses to familiar and unfamiliar human and dog odors , 2015, Behavioural Processes.

[19]  R. Dolan,et al.  Distinct spatial frequency sensitivities for processing faces and emotional expressions , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.

[20]  D. Reby,et al.  Orienting Asymmetries in Dogs’ Responses to Different Communicatory Components of Human Speech , 2014, Current Biology.

[21]  N. Bresolin,et al.  Correlation of Circulating CD133+ Progenitor Subclasses with a Mild Phenotype in Duchenne Muscular Dystrophy Patients , 2008, PloS one.

[22]  G. Rhodes,et al.  A comparative view of face perception. , 2010, Journal of comparative psychology.

[23]  G. Berns,et al.  Functional MRI in Awake Unrestrained Dogs , 2012, PloS one.

[24]  V Csányi,et al.  Attachment behavior in dogs (Canis familiaris): a new application of Ainsworth's (1969) Strange Situation Test. , 1998, Journal of comparative psychology.

[25]  M. Nagasawa,et al.  Dogs can discriminate human smiling faces from blank expressions , 2011, Animal Cognition.

[26]  Y. Sugita Face perception in monkeys reared with no exposure to faces , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[27]  Pier Francesco Ferrari,et al.  Towards a bottom-up perspective on animal and human cognition , 2010, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[28]  Daniel D. Dilks,et al.  Awake fMRI reveals a specialized region in dog temporal cortex for face processing , 2015, PeerJ.

[29]  Doris Y. Tsao,et al.  Faces and objects in macaque cerebral cortex , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.

[30]  Daniel Mills,et al.  Discrimination of human and dog faces and inversion responses in domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) , 2010, Animal Cognition.

[31]  Michael Tomasello,et al.  Human-like social skills in dogs? , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[32]  R. Adolphs Neural systems for recognizing emotion , 2002, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[33]  P. McGuire,et al.  Functional atlas of emotional faces processing: a voxel-based meta-analysis of 105 functional magnetic resonance imaging studies. , 2009, Journal of psychiatry & neuroscience : JPN.

[34]  Paolo Mongillo,et al.  Selective attention to humans in companion dogs, Canis familiaris , 2010, Animal Behaviour.

[35]  Á. Miklósi,et al.  Are readers of our face readers of our minds? Dogs (Canis familiaris) show situation-dependent recognition of human’s attention , 2004, Animal Cognition.

[36]  Stephen M. Smith,et al.  Functional MRI : an introduction to methods , 2002 .

[37]  Aleix M. Martinez,et al.  The AR face database , 1998 .

[38]  Keith J. Worsley,et al.  Statistical analysis of activation images , 2001 .

[39]  J. Peirce,et al.  Sheep don't forget a face , 2001, Nature.

[40]  Á. Miklósi,et al.  Voice-Sensitive Regions in the Dog and Human Brain Are Revealed by Comparative fMRI , 2014, Current Biology.

[41]  Xavier Palazzi,et al.  The Beagle Brain in Stereotaxic Coordinates , 2011 .

[42]  Nancy Kanwisher,et al.  Divide and conquer: A defense of functional localizers , 2006, NeuroImage.

[43]  G. Berns,et al.  Replicability and Heterogeneity of Awake Unrestrained Canine fMRI Responses , 2013, PloS one.

[44]  N. Kanwisher,et al.  The fusiform face area: a cortical region specialized for the perception of faces , 2006, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[45]  Brian B. Avants,et al.  A Digital Atlas of the Dog Brain , 2012, PloS one.

[46]  Takefumi Kikusui,et al.  Dog's gaze at its owner increases owner's urinary oxytocin during social interaction , 2009, Hormones and Behavior.

[47]  Á. Miklósi,et al.  Comparative social cognition: what can dogs teach us? , 2004, Animal Behaviour.

[48]  A. Stark,et al.  cis-Regulatory Requirements for Tissue-Specific Programs of the Circadian Clock , 2014, Current Biology.

[49]  L. Rogers,et al.  Hemispheric Specialization in Dogs for Processing Different Acoustic Stimuli , 2008, PloS one.

[50]  Á. Miklósi,et al.  Differential Sensitivity to Human Communication in Dogs, Wolves, and Human Infants , 2009, Science.

[51]  Jae-Jin Kim,et al.  Is a neutral face really evaluated as being emotionally neutral? , 2008, Psychiatry Research.