Sensory redundancy and perceptual invariance in force production and object manipulation

Many real-world tasks involve exploratory movements for the manipulation of objects with a wide range of physical properties. Historically, neurophysiological research has emphasized how force sensations and perceptions arise due to descending motor commands and ascending afferent feedback from proprioceptors. However, how force sensation and perception contribute to stable percepts of handheld objects’ properties, such as moments of mass (moment, moment of inertia, etc.), to facilitate smooth interaction and manipulation is unclear. Studies have shown that wielding movements, and the associated Ia re-afferent activity, may be necessary to perceive a handheld object’s moments of mass. Recent theoretical frameworks provide a novel basis to rethink how somatosensory information from different mechanoreceptors could be integrated with other sensory modalities, such as vision, to create stable percepts of object affordances associated with different moments of mass.

[1]  D. McCloskey,et al.  Estimation of weights and tensions and apparent involvement of a "sense of effort". , 1974, Experimental neurology.

[2]  Jeffrey B Wagman,et al.  Haptically creating affordances: the user-tool interface. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Applied.

[3]  J. Randall Flanagan,et al.  Coding and use of tactile signals from the fingertips in object manipulation tasks , 2009, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[4]  Madhur Mangalam,et al.  Multifractal signatures of perceptual processing on anatomical sleeves of the human body , 2020, Journal of the Royal Society Interface.

[5]  Alen Hajnal,et al.  Transfer of calibration between hand and foot: Functional equivalence and fractal fluctuations , 2011, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[6]  Eric L. Amazeen,et al.  Peripheral neuropathy and object length perception by effortful (dynamic) touch: A case study , 2006, Neuroscience Letters.

[7]  Uwe Proske,et al.  The illusion of changed position and movement from vibrating one arm is altered by vision or movement of the other arm , 2010, The Journal of physiology.

[8]  Kan Arai,et al.  Tactile force perception depends on the visual speed of the collision object. , 2009, Journal of vision.

[9]  C. Michaels Affordances: Four Points of Debate , 2003, How Shall Affordances be Refined? Four Perspectives.

[10]  M. Turvey Action and perception at the level of synergies. , 2007, Human movement science.

[11]  M. Latash,et al.  Perceptual and Motor Effects of Muscle Co-activation in a Force Production Task , 2020, Neuroscience.

[12]  M T Turvey,et al.  Principles of Part–Whole Selective Perception by Dynamic Touch Extend to the Torso , 2011, Journal of motor behavior.

[13]  Lena H. Ting,et al.  Force encoding in muscle spindles during stretch of passive muscle , 2017, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[14]  Charles Capaday,et al.  Pointing to One's Moving Hand: Putative Internal Models Do Not Contribute to Proprioceptive Acuity , 2018, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[15]  T. Nichols The Stresses and Strains of Tensegrity , 2014, Journal of motor behavior.

[16]  E. Amazeen,et al.  Lift speed moderates the effects of muscle activity on perceived heaviness , 2018, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[17]  三嶋 博之 The theory of affordances , 2008 .

[18]  Mark L. Latash,et al.  Exploring the Concept of Iso-perceptual Manifold (IPM): A Study of Finger Force-Matching Tasks , 2019, Neuroscience.

[19]  T. Higuchi,et al.  Turning perception on its head: cephalic perception of whole and partial length of a wielded object , 2016, Experimental Brain Research.

[20]  E. Holst,et al.  Das Reafferenzprinzip , 2004, Naturwissenschaften.

[21]  J. Wagman,et al.  Task specificity and anatomical independence in perception of properties by means of a wielded object. , 2014, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[22]  Jeffrey B. Wagman,et al.  Affordances and Inertial Constraints on Tool Use , 2001 .

[23]  Damian G. Kelty-Stephen,et al.  Interwoven fluctuations during intermodal perception: fractality in head sway supports the use of visual feedback in haptic perceptual judgments by manual wielding. , 2014, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[24]  Mark L. Latash,et al.  Stability of Kinesthetic Perception in Efferent-Afferent Spaces: The Concept of Iso-perceptual Manifold , 2017, Neuroscience.

[25]  Patrick S. Jensen,et al.  Surgical Forces and Tactile Perception During Retinal Microsurgery , 1999, MICCAI.

[26]  Michael T Turvey,et al.  The Medium of Haptic Perception: A Tensegrity Hypothesis , 2014, Journal of motor behavior.

[27]  M T Turvey,et al.  Haptic probing: Perceiving the length of a probe and the distance of a surface probed , 1992, Perception & psychophysics.

[28]  Michael T. Turvey,et al.  Effortful touch with minimal movement , 1992 .

[29]  C. Michaels Information, Perception, and Action: What Should Ecological Psychologists Learn From Milner and Goodale (1995)? , 2000 .

[30]  Madhur Mangalam,et al.  Global broadcasting of local fractal fluctuations in a bodywide distributed system supports perception via effortful touch , 2020 .

[31]  Satoshi Tadokoro,et al.  Presenting virtual stiffness by modulating the perceived force profile with suction pressure , 2014, 2014 IEEE Haptics Symposium (HAPTICS).

[32]  S. Levin Our Internal Universe , 2014, Journal of motor behavior.

[33]  Tarkeshwar Singh,et al.  Muscular effort differentially mediates perception of heaviness and length via dynamic touch , 2018, Experimental Brain Research.

[34]  Brian L Day,et al.  The fusimotor and reafferent origin of the sense of force and weight , 2011, The Journal of physiology.

[35]  J. A. Pruszynski,et al.  Edge-orientation processing in first-order tactile neurons , 2014, Nature Neuroscience.

[36]  Mark L Latash,et al.  Finger Force Matching and Verbal Reports: Testing Predictions of the Iso-Perceptual Manifold Concept , 2020, Journal of motor behavior.

[37]  Marco Davare,et al.  Sensorimotor Memory Biases Weight Perception During Object Lifting , 2015, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[38]  Barry C. Stillman,et al.  Making Sense of Proprioception , 2002 .

[39]  D. McCloskey,et al.  Sensations of heaviness. , 1977, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[40]  H. Bastian Sensation and Perception.—I , 1869, Nature.

[41]  Eric L. Amazeen,et al.  Perceived Heaviness Is Influenced by the Style of Lifting , 2011 .

[42]  C. Pagano,et al.  Constancy in Dynamic Touch: Length Perceived by Dynamic Touch Is Invariant Over Changes in Media , 2003 .

[43]  Peter J Beek,et al.  Which mechanical invariants are associated with the perception of length and heaviness of a nonvisible handheld rod? Testing the inertia tensor hypothesis. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[44]  M T Turvey,et al.  Obtaining information by dynamic (effortful) touching , 2011, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[45]  A. Goodwin,et al.  Sensory signals in neural populations underlying tactile perception and manipulation. , 2004, Annual review of neuroscience.

[46]  E. Cafarelli,et al.  Effect of vibration on static force sensation in man , 1981, Experimental Neurology.

[47]  K. Newell,et al.  Perception of the length of an object through dynamic touch is invariant across changes in the medium , 2017, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[48]  V. Macefield,et al.  Vibration sensitivity of human muscle spindles and golgi tendon organs , 2007, Muscle & nerve.

[49]  Steven J. Harrison,et al.  Comparison of Dynamic (Effortful) Touch by Hand and Foot , 2007, Journal of motor behavior.

[50]  I. W. Hunter,et al.  Effect of fatigue on force sensation , 1983, Experimental Neurology.

[51]  Justin M. Fine,et al.  Perceived heaviness in the context of Newton's Second Law: Combined effects of muscle activity and lifting kinematics. , 2016, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[52]  T. Nichols,et al.  Short-Term Effect of Crural Fasciotomy on Kinematic Variability and Propulsion During Level Locomotion , 2014, Journal of motor behavior.

[53]  F. Mechsner,et al.  The Bodywide Fascial Network as a Sensory Organ for Haptic Perception , 2014, Journal of motor behavior.

[54]  Peter J Beek,et al.  Mechanical invariants are implicated in dynamic touch as a function of their salience in the stimulus flow. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[55]  I. Kingma,et al.  The Inertia Tensor Versus Static Moment and Mass in Perceiving Length and Heaviness of Hand-Wielded Rods , 2002 .

[56]  Steven J. Harrison,et al.  Perceiving action-relevant properties of tools through dynamic touch: effects of mass distribution, exploration style, and intention. , 2011, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[57]  John van der Kamp,et al.  The Media for Haptic Perception and for Force Transmission in Movement Are the Same of Course , 2014, Journal of motor behavior.

[58]  Claudia Carello,et al.  Haptic selective attention by foot and by hand , 2007, Neuroscience Letters.

[59]  Simon C Gandevia,et al.  Proprioception, Tensegrity, and Motor Control , 2014, Journal of motor behavior.

[60]  Damian G. Kelty-Stephen,et al.  Fractal fluctuations in muscular activity contribute to judgments of length but not heaviness via dynamic touch , 2019, Experimental Brain Research.

[61]  Michael T Turvey,et al.  Bernstein's levels of movement construction: A contemporary perspective. , 2018, Human movement science.

[62]  R. Sperry Neural basis of the spontaneous optokinetic response produced by visual inversion. , 1950, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[63]  Kenneth O. Johnson,et al.  Neural Basis of Haptic Perception , 2002 .

[64]  S. Gandevia,et al.  The proprioceptive senses: their roles in signaling body shape, body position and movement, and muscle force. , 2012, Physiological reviews.

[65]  J. Lackner,et al.  Changes in apparent body orientation and sensory localization induced by vibration of postural muscles: vibratory myesthetic illusions. , 1979, Aviation, space, and environmental medicine.

[66]  Damian G. Kelty-Stephen,et al.  Bodywide fluctuations support manual exploration: Fractal fluctuations in posture predict perception of heaviness and length via effortful touch by the hand. , 2019, Human movement science.

[67]  Satoshi Tadokoro,et al.  Enhancement of human force perception by multi-point tactile stimulation , 2011, 2011 IEEE/RSJ International Conference on Intelligent Robots and Systems.

[68]  M. Latash,et al.  Force illusions and drifts observed during muscle vibration. , 2018, Journal of neurophysiology.

[69]  Valérie Hospod,et al.  Fusimotor drive may adjust muscle spindle feedback to task requirements in humans. , 2009, Journal of neurophysiology.

[70]  E. Holst Relations between the central Nervous System and the peripheral organs , 1954 .

[71]  S. R. Ganeshan,et al.  Effortful touch with minimum movement: revisited. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[72]  M T Turvey,et al.  Tensorial basis to the constancy of perceived object extent over variations of dynamic touch , 1993, Perception & psychophysics.

[73]  Damian G. Stephen,et al.  The role of fractality in perceptual learning: exploration in dynamic touch. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[74]  H. C. Dijkerman,et al.  Somatosensory processes subserving perception and action , 2007, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[75]  U. Proske,et al.  The neural basis of the senses of effort, force and heaviness , 2019, Experimental Brain Research.

[76]  J. Flanagan,et al.  Independence of perceptual and sensorimotor predictions in the size–weight illusion , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[77]  M. Goodale,et al.  Two distinct modes of control for object-directed action. , 2004, Progress in brain research.