Acquisition of automatic imitation is sensitive to sensorimotor contingency.

The associative sequence learning model proposes that the development of the mirror system depends on the same mechanisms of associative learning that mediate Pavlovian and instrumental conditioning. To test this model, two experiments used the reduction of automatic imitation through incompatible sensorimotor training to assess whether mirror system plasticity is sensitive to contingency (i.e., the extent to which activation of one representation predicts activation of another). In Experiment 1, residual automatic imitation was measured following incompatible training in which the action stimulus was a perfect predictor of the response (contingent) or not at all predictive of the response (noncontingent). A contingency effect was observed: There was less automatic imitation indicative of more learning in the contingent group. Experiment 2 replicated this contingency effect and showed that, as predicted by associative learning theory, it can be abolished by signaling trials in which the response occurs in the absence of an action stimulus. These findings support the view that mirror system development depends on associative learning and indicate that this learning is not purely Hebbian. If this is correct, associative learning theory could be used to explain, predict, and intervene in mirror system development.

[1]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Understanding motor events: a neurophysiological study , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[2]  S. Wise,et al.  Learning-dependent neuronal activity in the premotor cortex: activity during the acquisition of conditional motor associations , 1991, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[3]  Caroline Catmur,et al.  Making Mirrors: Premotor Cortex Stimulation Enhances Mirror and Counter-mirror Motor Facilitation , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[4]  J. Pearce,et al.  A model for Pavlovian learning: variations in the effectiveness of conditioned but not of unconditioned stimuli. , 1980, Psychological review.

[5]  Caroline Catmur,et al.  Associative sequence learning: the role of experience in the development of imitation and the mirror system , 2009, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[6]  A. Goldman,et al.  Mirror neurons and the simulation theory of mind-reading , 1998, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[7]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  The mirror-neuron system. , 2004, Annual review of neuroscience.

[8]  W. J. Griffiths,et al.  Free-Operant Acquisition with Delayed Reinforcement , 1992 .

[9]  V. Gallese The 'shared manifold' hypothesis: From mirror neurons to empathy. , 2001 .

[10]  C. Heyes,et al.  Experience modulates automatic imitation. , 2005, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[11]  R. Passingham,et al.  Seeing or Doing? Influence of Visual and Motor Familiarity in Action Observation , 2006, Current Biology.

[12]  Nadim Joni Shah,et al.  Prefrontal involvement in imitation learning of hand actions: Effects of practice and expertise , 2007, NeuroImage.

[13]  J. Mazziotta,et al.  Cortical mechanisms of human imitation. , 1999, Science.

[14]  A. Dickinson,et al.  Contingency Effects with Maintained Instrumental Reinforcement , 1985 .

[15]  E. Capaldi,et al.  The organization of behavior. , 1992, Journal of applied behavior analysis.

[16]  E. Altenmüller,et al.  Transmodal Sensorimotor Networks during Action Observation in Professional Pianists , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[17]  R. Rescorla Probability of shock in the presence and absence of CS in fear conditioning. , 1968, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[18]  C. Heyes,et al.  What Is the Significance of Imitation in Animals , 2000 .

[19]  C. Heyes,et al.  Hand to mouth: automatic imitation across effector systems. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[20]  C. Keysers,et al.  Towards a unifying neural theory of social cognition. , 2006, Progress in brain research.

[21]  C. Heyes,et al.  Imitation in infancy: the wealth of the stimulus. , 2011, Developmental science.

[22]  H. Bekkering,et al.  The mirror neuron system is more active during complementary compared with imitative action , 2007, Nature Neuroscience.

[23]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Motor facilitation during action observation: a magnetic stimulation study. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[24]  Caroline Catmur,et al.  Time course analyses confirm independence of imitative and spatial compatibility. , 2011, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[25]  D. Perrett,et al.  Opinion TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences Vol.8 No.11 November 2004 Demystifying social cognition: a Hebbian perspective , 2022 .

[26]  S. Pinker How the Mind Works , 1999, Philosophy after Darwin.

[27]  L. Tiedens,et al.  Power moves: complementarity in dominant and submissive nonverbal behavior. , 2003, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[28]  N. Mackintosh A Theory of Attention: Variations in the Associability of Stimuli with Reinforcement , 1975 .

[29]  A. Dickinson,et al.  Instrumental judgment and performance under variations in action-outcome contingency and contiguity , 1991, Memory & cognition.

[30]  R. Passingham,et al.  Premotor cortex and the conditions for movement in monkeys (Macaca fascicularis) , 1985, Behavioural Brain Research.

[31]  R. Rescorla A theory of pavlovian conditioning: The effectiveness of reinforcement and non-reinforcement , 1972 .

[32]  B. Hommel,et al.  Contiguity and contingency in action-effect learning , 2004, Psychological research.

[33]  A. Osman,et al.  Dimensional overlap: cognitive basis for stimulus-response compatibility--a model and taxonomy. , 1990, Psychological review.

[34]  D R Shanks,et al.  Selectional processes in causality judgment , 1989, Memory & cognition.

[35]  V. Gallese The ‘‘shared manifold’’ hypothesis: from mirror neurons to empathy , 2001 .

[36]  R. Passingham,et al.  Action observation and acquired motor skills: an FMRI study with expert dancers. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.

[37]  H. Bekkering,et al.  Understanding action beyond imitation: reversed compatibility effects of action observation in imitation and joint action. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[38]  Marcel Brass,et al.  Experience-based priming of body parts: A study of action imitation , 2008, Brain Research.

[39]  W. F. Prokasy,et al.  Classical conditioning II: Current research and theory. , 1972 .

[40]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Dorsal premotor cortex and conditional movement selection: A PET functional mapping study. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[41]  D. Perrett,et al.  Responses of Anterior Superior Temporal Polysensory (STPa) Neurons to Biological Motion Stimuli , 1994, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[42]  W. Kintsch,et al.  Memory and cognition , 1977 .

[43]  Ralph R. Miller,et al.  Information processing in animals : memory mechanisms , 1983 .

[44]  Marcel Brass,et al.  Through the looking glass: counter‐mirror activation following incompatible sensorimotor learning , 2008, The European journal of neuroscience.

[45]  R. E Passingham,et al.  Activations related to “mirror” and “canonical” neurones in the human brain: an fMRI study , 2003, NeuroImage.

[46]  M. Petrides The effect of periarcuate lesions in the monkey on the performance of symmetrically and asymmetrically reinforced visual and auditory go, no- go tasks , 1986, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[47]  C. Heyes,et al.  Automatic imitation of intransitive actions , 2008, Brain and Cognition.

[48]  C. Heyes Where do mirror neurons come from? , 2010, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[49]  T. Paus,et al.  Modulation of cortical excitability during action observation: a transcranial magnetic stimulation study , 2000, Neuroreport.

[50]  C. Heyes,et al.  Sensorimotor experience enhances automatic imitation of robotic action , 2007, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[51]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Premotor cortex and the recognition of motor actions. , 1996, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[52]  U. Dimberg,et al.  Facial reactions to emotional stimuli: Automatically controlled emotional responses , 2002 .

[53]  W. Prinz,et al.  Movement observation affects movement execution in a simple response task. , 2001, Acta psychologica.

[54]  Caroline Catmur,et al.  Sensorimotor Learning Configures the Human Mirror System , 2007, Current Biology.

[55]  Matthew R Longo,et al.  Imitative response tendencies following observation of intransitive actions. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[56]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Action recognition in the premotor cortex. , 1996, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[57]  C. Heyes Causes and consequences of imitation , 2001, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.