Acquisition of automatic imitation is sensitive to sensorimotor contingency.
暂无分享,去创建一个
Cecilia Heyes | Clare Press | Richard Cook | Anthony Dickinson | A. Dickinson | C. Heyes | Clare Press | R. Cook
[1] G. Rizzolatti,et al. Understanding motor events: a neurophysiological study , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.
[2] S. Wise,et al. Learning-dependent neuronal activity in the premotor cortex: activity during the acquisition of conditional motor associations , 1991, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.
[3] Caroline Catmur,et al. Making Mirrors: Premotor Cortex Stimulation Enhances Mirror and Counter-mirror Motor Facilitation , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[4] J. Pearce,et al. A model for Pavlovian learning: variations in the effectiveness of conditioned but not of unconditioned stimuli. , 1980, Psychological review.
[5] Caroline Catmur,et al. Associative sequence learning: the role of experience in the development of imitation and the mirror system , 2009, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[6] A. Goldman,et al. Mirror neurons and the simulation theory of mind-reading , 1998, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[7] G. Rizzolatti,et al. The mirror-neuron system. , 2004, Annual review of neuroscience.
[8] W. J. Griffiths,et al. Free-Operant Acquisition with Delayed Reinforcement , 1992 .
[9] V. Gallese. The 'shared manifold' hypothesis: From mirror neurons to empathy. , 2001 .
[10] C. Heyes,et al. Experience modulates automatic imitation. , 2005, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.
[11] R. Passingham,et al. Seeing or Doing? Influence of Visual and Motor Familiarity in Action Observation , 2006, Current Biology.
[12] Nadim Joni Shah,et al. Prefrontal involvement in imitation learning of hand actions: Effects of practice and expertise , 2007, NeuroImage.
[13] J. Mazziotta,et al. Cortical mechanisms of human imitation. , 1999, Science.
[14] A. Dickinson,et al. Contingency Effects with Maintained Instrumental Reinforcement , 1985 .
[15] E. Capaldi,et al. The organization of behavior. , 1992, Journal of applied behavior analysis.
[16] E. Altenmüller,et al. Transmodal Sensorimotor Networks during Action Observation in Professional Pianists , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[17] R. Rescorla. Probability of shock in the presence and absence of CS in fear conditioning. , 1968, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.
[18] C. Heyes,et al. What Is the Significance of Imitation in Animals , 2000 .
[19] C. Heyes,et al. Hand to mouth: automatic imitation across effector systems. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[20] C. Keysers,et al. Towards a unifying neural theory of social cognition. , 2006, Progress in brain research.
[21] C. Heyes,et al. Imitation in infancy: the wealth of the stimulus. , 2011, Developmental science.
[22] H. Bekkering,et al. The mirror neuron system is more active during complementary compared with imitative action , 2007, Nature Neuroscience.
[23] G. Rizzolatti,et al. Motor facilitation during action observation: a magnetic stimulation study. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.
[24] Caroline Catmur,et al. Time course analyses confirm independence of imitative and spatial compatibility. , 2011, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[25] D. Perrett,et al. Opinion TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences Vol.8 No.11 November 2004 Demystifying social cognition: a Hebbian perspective , 2022 .
[26] S. Pinker. How the Mind Works , 1999, Philosophy after Darwin.
[27] L. Tiedens,et al. Power moves: complementarity in dominant and submissive nonverbal behavior. , 2003, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[28] N. Mackintosh. A Theory of Attention: Variations in the Associability of Stimuli with Reinforcement , 1975 .
[29] A. Dickinson,et al. Instrumental judgment and performance under variations in action-outcome contingency and contiguity , 1991, Memory & cognition.
[30] R. Passingham,et al. Premotor cortex and the conditions for movement in monkeys (Macaca fascicularis) , 1985, Behavioural Brain Research.
[31] R. Rescorla. A theory of pavlovian conditioning: The effectiveness of reinforcement and non-reinforcement , 1972 .
[32] B. Hommel,et al. Contiguity and contingency in action-effect learning , 2004, Psychological research.
[33] A. Osman,et al. Dimensional overlap: cognitive basis for stimulus-response compatibility--a model and taxonomy. , 1990, Psychological review.
[34] D R Shanks,et al. Selectional processes in causality judgment , 1989, Memory & cognition.
[35] V. Gallese. The ‘‘shared manifold’’ hypothesis: from mirror neurons to empathy , 2001 .
[36] R. Passingham,et al. Action observation and acquired motor skills: an FMRI study with expert dancers. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.
[37] H. Bekkering,et al. Understanding action beyond imitation: reversed compatibility effects of action observation in imitation and joint action. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[38] Marcel Brass,et al. Experience-based priming of body parts: A study of action imitation , 2008, Brain Research.
[39] W. F. Prokasy,et al. Classical conditioning II: Current research and theory. , 1972 .
[40] Scott T. Grafton,et al. Dorsal premotor cortex and conditional movement selection: A PET functional mapping study. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.
[41] D. Perrett,et al. Responses of Anterior Superior Temporal Polysensory (STPa) Neurons to Biological Motion Stimuli , 1994, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[42] W. Kintsch,et al. Memory and cognition , 1977 .
[43] Ralph R. Miller,et al. Information processing in animals : memory mechanisms , 1983 .
[44] Marcel Brass,et al. Through the looking glass: counter‐mirror activation following incompatible sensorimotor learning , 2008, The European journal of neuroscience.
[45] R. E Passingham,et al. Activations related to “mirror” and “canonical” neurones in the human brain: an fMRI study , 2003, NeuroImage.
[46] M. Petrides. The effect of periarcuate lesions in the monkey on the performance of symmetrically and asymmetrically reinforced visual and auditory go, no- go tasks , 1986, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.
[47] C. Heyes,et al. Automatic imitation of intransitive actions , 2008, Brain and Cognition.
[48] C. Heyes. Where do mirror neurons come from? , 2010, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.
[49] T. Paus,et al. Modulation of cortical excitability during action observation: a transcranial magnetic stimulation study , 2000, Neuroreport.
[50] C. Heyes,et al. Sensorimotor experience enhances automatic imitation of robotic action , 2007, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[51] G. Rizzolatti,et al. Premotor cortex and the recognition of motor actions. , 1996, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.
[52] U. Dimberg,et al. Facial reactions to emotional stimuli: Automatically controlled emotional responses , 2002 .
[53] W. Prinz,et al. Movement observation affects movement execution in a simple response task. , 2001, Acta psychologica.
[54] Caroline Catmur,et al. Sensorimotor Learning Configures the Human Mirror System , 2007, Current Biology.
[55] Matthew R Longo,et al. Imitative response tendencies following observation of intransitive actions. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[56] G. Rizzolatti,et al. Action recognition in the premotor cortex. , 1996, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[57] C. Heyes. Causes and consequences of imitation , 2001, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.