Subconscious processing reveals dissociable contextual modulations of visual size perception
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Y. Sugita,et al. Size-contrast illusion induced by unconscious context. , 2018, Journal of vision.
[2] D. Shanks. Regressive research: The pitfalls of post hoc data selection in the study of unconscious mental processes , 2016, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[3] Johannes Jacobus Fahrenfort,et al. Perceptual integration without conscious access , 2016, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[4] Marius V Peelen,et al. Privileged access to awareness for faces and objects of expertise. , 2016, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[5] Johan Wagemans,et al. Scene Integration Without Awareness , 2016, Psychological science.
[6] D. Schwarzkopf,et al. Induction of Kanizsa Contours Requires Awareness of the Inducing Context , 2016, bioRxiv.
[7] I. Murakami,et al. Facilitation of contrast detection by flankers without perceived orientation. , 2015, Journal of vision.
[8] Marius V Peelen,et al. Journal of Experimental Psychology : General Content-Specific Expectations Enhance Stimulus Detectability by Increasing Perceptual Sensitivity , 2015 .
[9] Jifan Zhou,et al. The working memory Ponzo illusion: Involuntary integration of visuospatial information stored in visual working memory , 2015, Cognition.
[10] Miguel A. Vadillo,et al. Underpowered samples, false negatives, and unconscious learning , 2015, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[11] T. Jaeger,et al. The Ebbinghaus Illusion: New Contextual Effects and Theoretical Considerations , 2015, Perceptual and motor skills.
[12] L. Albertazzi,et al. The Ebbinghaus illusion in a fish (Xenotoca eiseni) , 2014, Animal Cognition.
[13] D. Samuel Schwarzkopf,et al. Effective Connectivity within Human Primary Visual Cortex Predicts Interindividual Diversity in Illusory Perception , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[14] M. Morrone,et al. Blood Oxygen Level-Dependent Activation of the Primary Visual Cortex Predicts Size Adaptation Illusion , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[15] Geraint Rees,et al. Variability in visual cortex size reflects tradeoff between local orientation sensitivity and global orientation modulation , 2013, Nature Communications.
[16] L. Regolin,et al. Perception of the Ebbinghaus illusion in four-day-old domestic chicks (Gallus gallus) , 2013, Animal Cognition.
[17] Victor A. F. Lamme,et al. Confuse Your Illusion , 2013, Psychological science.
[18] T. Murayama. Relative Size Discrimination and Perception of the Ebbinghaus Illusion in a Bottlenose Dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) , 2012 .
[19] Colin W G Clifford,et al. Dynamics of unconscious contextual effects in orientation processing , 2012, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[20] Melvyn A. Goodale,et al. Retinotopic activity in V1 reflects the perceived and not the retinal size of an afterimage , 2012, Nature Neuroscience.
[21] Geraint Rees,et al. Interocular induction of illusory size perception , 2011, BMC Neuroscience.
[22] D. Schwarzkopf,et al. Contextual Illusions Reveal the Limit of Unconscious Visual Processing , 2011, Psychological science.
[23] D. Samuel Schwarzkopf,et al. The surface area of human V1 predicts the subjective experience of object size , 2010, Nature Neuroscience.
[24] Jeffrey N. Rouder,et al. Bayesian t tests for accepting and rejecting the null hypothesis , 2009, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[25] D. Kersten,et al. Attention-Dependent Representation of a Size Illusion in Human V1 , 2008, Current Biology.
[26] R. Blake,et al. Fearful expressions gain preferential access to awareness during continuous flash suppression. , 2007, Emotion.
[27] D. Kersten,et al. Responses to Lightness Variations in Early Human Visual Cortex , 2007, Current Biology.
[28] Sheng He,et al. Processing of Invisible Stimuli: Advantage of Upright Faces and Recognizable Words in Overcoming Interocular Suppression , 2007, Psychological science.
[29] Sheng He,et al. Cortical Responses to Invisible Faces: Dissociating Subsystems for Facial-Information Processing , 2006, Current Biology.
[30] D. Kersten,et al. The representation of perceived angular size in human primary visual cortex , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.
[31] C. Clifford,et al. Suppressed Patterns Alter Vision during Binocular Rivalry , 2005, Current Biology.
[32] C. Koch,et al. Continuous flash suppression reduces negative afterimages , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.
[33] Justin A. Harris,et al. Contextual Modulation outside of Awareness , 2005, Current Biology.
[34] Kenith V. Sobel,et al. Subjective contours and binocular rivalry suppression , 2003, Vision Research.
[35] William Hodos,et al. Size-threshold changes after lesions of the visual telencephalon in pigeons , 1986, Behavioural Brain Research.
[36] K. Macko,et al. Near-field acuity changes after visual system lesions in pigeons. II. Telencephalon , 1984, Behavioural Brain Research.
[37] K. Macko,et al. Near-field acuity after visual system lesions in pigeons. I. Thalamus , 1984, Behavioural Brain Research.
[38] T Jaeger,et al. Ebbinghaus illusions: Size contrast or contour interaction phenomena? , 1978, Perception & psychophysics.
[39] Stanley Coren,et al. Size contrast as a function of figural similarity , 1974 .
[40] D W Massaro,et al. Judgmental model of the Ebbinghaus illusion. , 1971, Journal of experimental psychology.
[41] L. Cooper,et al. Delboeuf-type circle illusions: interactions among luminance, temporal characteristics, and inducing-figure variations. , 1970, Journal of experimental psychology.
[42] D. Schwarzkopf,et al. Induction of Kanizsa contours requires awareness 1 , 2016 .
[43] P. Bressan,et al. Going round in circles: shape effects in the Ebbinghaus illusion. , 2002, Spatial vision.
[44] D H Brainard,et al. The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.
[45] D G Pelli,et al. The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. , 1997, Spatial vision.
[46] Robert H. Pollack,et al. Effect of contrast level and temporal order on the Ebbinghaus circles illusion , 1977 .