Proactive control of irrelevant task rules during cued task switching

AbstractIn task-switching paradigms, participants are often slower on incongruent than congruent trials, a pattern known as the task-rule congruency effect. This effect suggests that irrelevant task rules or associated responses may be retrieved automatically in spite of task cues. The purpose of the present study was to examine whether the task-rule congruency effect may be modulated via manipulations intended to induce variation in proactive control. Manipulating the proportion of congruent to incongruent trials strongly influenced the magnitude of the task-rule congruency effect. The effect was significantly reduced in a mostly incongruent list relative to a mostly congruent list, a pattern that was observed for not only biased but also 50 % congruent items. This finding implicates a role for global attentional control processes in the task-rule congruency effect. In contrast, enhancing the preparation of relevant (cued) task rules by the provision of a monetary incentive substantially reduced mixing costs but did not affect the task-rule congruency effect. These patterns support the view that there may be multiple routes by which proactive control can influence task-switching performance; however, only select routes appear to influence the automatic retrieval of irrelevant task rules.

[1]  Koji Jimura,et al.  Age-related shifts in brain activity dynamics during task switching. , 2010, Cerebral cortex.

[2]  T. Goschke Intentional reconfiguration and involuntary persistence in task-set switching , 2000 .

[3]  I. Koch,et al.  The role of response selection for inhibition of task sets in task shifting. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[4]  G. Baylis,et al.  Effects of increased response dominance and contextual disintegration on the Stroop interference effect in older adults. , 1998, Psychology and aging.

[5]  Punit Shah Toward a Neurobiology of Unrealistic Optimism , 2012, Front. Psychology.

[6]  J Tzelgov,et al.  Specifying the relations between automaticity and consciousness: a theoretical note. , 1997, Consciousness and cognition.

[7]  Stephen Monsell,et al.  Can the task-cuing paradigm measure an endogenous task-set reconfiguration process? , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[8]  Yoav Kessler,et al.  The reaction-time task-rule congruency effect is not affected by working memory load: further support for the activated long-term memory hypothesis , 2010, Psychological research.

[9]  Andrea M Philipp,et al.  Control and interference in task switching--a review. , 2010, Psychological bulletin.

[10]  Julie M Bugg Dissociating Levels of Cognitive Control , 2012 .

[11]  Hannah S. Locke,et al.  Prefrontal cortex mediation of cognitive enhancement in rewarding motivational contexts , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[12]  Stephen Monsell,et al.  Task-set reconfiguration with predictable and unpredictable task switches , 2003, Memory & cognition.

[13]  Richard P. Heitz,et al.  The speed-accuracy tradeoff: history, physiology, methodology, and behavior , 2014, Front. Neurosci..

[14]  Andrew R. A. Conway,et al.  Variation in working memory , 2008 .

[15]  Christian Frings,et al.  To be or not to be...included in an event file: integration and retrieval of distractors in stimulus-response episodes is influenced by perceptual grouping. , 2011, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[16]  G. Logan Attention and automaticity in Stroop and priming tasks: Theory and data , 1980, Cognitive Psychology.

[17]  James R. Schmidt Temporal Learning and List-Level Proportion Congruency: Conflict Adaptation or Learning When to Respond? , 2013, PloS one.

[18]  T. Braver,et al.  Explaining the many varieties of working memory variation: Dual mechanisms of cognitive control. , 2007 .

[19]  R. Bakeman Recommended effect size statistics for repeated measures designs , 2005, Behavior research methods.

[20]  S. Monsell,et al.  Costs of a predictible switch between simple cognitive tasks. , 1995 .

[21]  Yoav Kessler,et al.  The task rule congruency effect in task switching reflects activated long-term memory. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[22]  B. Hommel Spontaneous decay of response-code activation , 1994, Psychological research.

[23]  M. Botvinick,et al.  Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. , 2001, Psychological review.

[24]  N. Meiran,et al.  On the origins of the task mixing cost in the cuing task-switching paradigm. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[25]  N. Meiran Task Rule-Congruency and Simon-Like Effects in Switching between Spatial Tasks , 2005, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[26]  N. Meiran Modeling cognitive control in task-switching , 2000, Psychological research.

[27]  R De Jong,et al.  An intention-activation account of residual switch costs , 2000 .

[28]  R. Kliegl,et al.  Task-set switching and long-term memory retrieval. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[29]  Darryl W. Schneider Isolating a mediated route for response congruency effects in task switching. , 2015, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[30]  D. Alan Allport,et al.  SHIFTING INTENTIONAL SET - EXPLORING THE DYNAMIC CONTROL OF TASKS , 1994 .

[31]  The target of task switching. , 2010, Canadian journal of experimental psychology = Revue canadienne de psychologie experimentale.

[32]  J. Driver,et al.  Control of Cognitive Processes: Attention and Performance XVIII , 2000 .

[33]  Yoav Kessler,et al.  Control by action representation and input selection (CARIS): a theoretical framework for task switching , 2008, Psychological research.

[34]  Rethinking the role of automaticity in cognitive control , 2012, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[35]  G. Logan,et al.  When it helps to be misled: Facilitative effects of increasing the frequency of conflicting stimuli in a Stroop-like task , 1979 .

[36]  L. Jacoby,et al.  Why it is too early to lose control in accounts of item-specific proportion congruency effects. , 2011, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[37]  James R. Schmidt,et al.  List-level transfer effects in temporal learning: Further complications for the list-level proportion congruent effect , 2014 .

[38]  Keith A Hutchison,et al.  Converging evidence for control of color-word Stroop interference at the item level. , 2013, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[39]  Gordon D. Logan,et al.  Strategies in the color-word Stroop task , 1984 .

[40]  E. Donchin,et al.  Optimizing the use of information: strategic control of activation of responses. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[41]  Mike Wendt,et al.  The impact of stimulus-specific practice and task instructions on response congruency effects between tasks , 2008, Psychological research.

[42]  Todd S. Braver,et al.  A model of dual control mechanisms through anterior cingulate and prefrontal cortex interactions , 2006, Neurocomputing.

[43]  Jeffrey P. Toth,et al.  Dissociation of Processes Underlying Spatial S-R Compatibility: Evidence for the Independent Influence of What and Where , 1995, Consciousness and Cognition.

[44]  D. Algom,et al.  Driven by information: a tectonic theory of Stroop effects. , 2003, Psychological review.

[45]  Darryl W. Schneider,et al.  Modeling task switching without switching tasks: a short-term priming account of explicitly cued performance. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[46]  J. Stroop Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. , 1992 .

[47]  Emily R. Cohen-Shikora,et al.  Expectations and experience: Dissociable bases for cognitive control? , 2015, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[48]  Matthew J. C. Crump,et al.  In Support of a Distinction between Voluntary and Stimulus-Driven Control: A Review of the Literature on Proportion Congruent Effects , 2012, Front. Psychology.

[49]  Andrea Kiesel,et al.  Top-down versus bottom-up: when instructions overcome automatic retrieval , 2013, Psychological research.

[50]  Chris Blais,et al.  Behavioral and Neural Evidence for Item-specific Performance Monitoring , 2010, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[51]  D. A. Taylor,et al.  The cuing and priming of cognitive operations. , 1987, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[52]  N. Meiran,et al.  Conflict control in task conflict and response conflict , 2014, Psychological Research.

[53]  D. Lowe,et al.  Selective and divided Attention in a Stroop task. , 1982, Canadian journal of psychology.

[54]  James R. Schmidt,et al.  The Stroop effect: why proportion congruent has nothing to do with congruency and everything to do with contingency. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[55]  Stephen Monsell,et al.  Residual costs in task switching: Testing the failure-to-engage hypothesis , 2002, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[56]  Robert W. Proctor,et al.  Automaticity without extensive training: The role of memory retrieval in implementation of task-defined rules , 2011, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[57]  J. Bugg,et al.  Conflict-triggered top-down control: default mode, last resort, or no such thing? , 2014, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[58]  J. Algina,et al.  Generalized eta and omega squared statistics: measures of effect size for some common research designs. , 2003, Psychological methods.

[59]  A. Kiesel,et al.  Task switching: on the origin of response congruency effects , 2007, Psychological research.

[60]  Andrea M Philipp,et al.  The role of inhibition in task switching: A review , 2010, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[61]  J. Bugg,et al.  List-wide control is not entirely elusive: Evidence from picture–word Stroop , 2011, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[62]  A. Vandierendonck,et al.  Task switching: interplay of reconfiguration and interference control. , 2010, Psychological bulletin.

[63]  Colin M. Macleod Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: an integrative review. , 1991, Psychological bulletin.

[64]  B. Hommel,et al.  Task-switching and long-term priming: Role of episodic stimulus–task bindings in task-shift costs , 2003, Cognitive Psychology.

[65]  T. Egner Congruency sequence effects and cognitive control , 2007, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[66]  Derek Besner,et al.  Item-specific adaptation and the conflict-monitoring hypothesis: a computational model. , 2007, Psychological review.

[67]  S. Hsieh,et al.  Resolving task rule incongruence during task switching by competitor rule suppression. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[68]  K. Hutchison,et al.  The Interactive Effects of Listwide Control, Item-based Control, and Working Memory Capacity on Stroop Performance and Holly Vander Vos for Scheduling and Running Participants for This Study. I Am Also Grateful To , 2022 .

[69]  Mike Wendt,et al.  Conflict-frequency affects flanker interference: role of stimulus-ensemble-specific practiceand flanker-response contingencies. , 2009, Experimental psychology.

[70]  Daniel Algom,et al.  The stroop effect: It is not the robust phenomenon that you have thought it to be , 2000, Memory & cognition.

[71]  L. Jacoby,et al.  Multiple levels of control in the Stroop task , 2008, Memory & cognition.

[72]  Kimberly S. Chiew,et al.  Temporal Dynamics of Motivation-Cognitive Control Interactions Revealed by High-Resolution Pupillometry , 2013, Front. Psychology.

[73]  F. Karayanidis,et al.  The many faces of preparatory control in task switching: Reviewing a decade of fMRI research , 2013, Human brain mapping.

[74]  W. Notebaert,et al.  Hebbian learning of cognitive control: dealing with specific and nonspecific adaptation. , 2008, Psychological review.

[75]  L. Jacoby,et al.  Stroop process dissociations: the relationship between facilitation and interference. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[76]  N. Meiran Reconfiguration of processing mode prior to task performance. , 1996 .

[77]  Mary Thomas You Can't Have It Both Ways: An Examination of Congruency Effects in Task Switching. , 2010 .

[78]  M. McDaniel,et al.  Revealing list-level control in the Stroop task by uncovering its benefits and a cost. , 2011, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.