Proactive control of irrelevant task rules during cued task switching
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Koji Jimura,et al. Age-related shifts in brain activity dynamics during task switching. , 2010, Cerebral cortex.
[2] T. Goschke. Intentional reconfiguration and involuntary persistence in task-set switching , 2000 .
[3] I. Koch,et al. The role of response selection for inhibition of task sets in task shifting. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[4] G. Baylis,et al. Effects of increased response dominance and contextual disintegration on the Stroop interference effect in older adults. , 1998, Psychology and aging.
[5] Punit Shah. Toward a Neurobiology of Unrealistic Optimism , 2012, Front. Psychology.
[6] J Tzelgov,et al. Specifying the relations between automaticity and consciousness: a theoretical note. , 1997, Consciousness and cognition.
[7] Stephen Monsell,et al. Can the task-cuing paradigm measure an endogenous task-set reconfiguration process? , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[8] Yoav Kessler,et al. The reaction-time task-rule congruency effect is not affected by working memory load: further support for the activated long-term memory hypothesis , 2010, Psychological research.
[9] Andrea M Philipp,et al. Control and interference in task switching--a review. , 2010, Psychological bulletin.
[10] Julie M Bugg. Dissociating Levels of Cognitive Control , 2012 .
[11] Hannah S. Locke,et al. Prefrontal cortex mediation of cognitive enhancement in rewarding motivational contexts , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[12] Stephen Monsell,et al. Task-set reconfiguration with predictable and unpredictable task switches , 2003, Memory & cognition.
[13] Richard P. Heitz,et al. The speed-accuracy tradeoff: history, physiology, methodology, and behavior , 2014, Front. Neurosci..
[14] Andrew R. A. Conway,et al. Variation in working memory , 2008 .
[15] Christian Frings,et al. To be or not to be...included in an event file: integration and retrieval of distractors in stimulus-response episodes is influenced by perceptual grouping. , 2011, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[16] G. Logan. Attention and automaticity in Stroop and priming tasks: Theory and data , 1980, Cognitive Psychology.
[17] James R. Schmidt. Temporal Learning and List-Level Proportion Congruency: Conflict Adaptation or Learning When to Respond? , 2013, PloS one.
[18] T. Braver,et al. Explaining the many varieties of working memory variation: Dual mechanisms of cognitive control. , 2007 .
[19] R. Bakeman. Recommended effect size statistics for repeated measures designs , 2005, Behavior research methods.
[20] S. Monsell,et al. Costs of a predictible switch between simple cognitive tasks. , 1995 .
[21] Yoav Kessler,et al. The task rule congruency effect in task switching reflects activated long-term memory. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[22] B. Hommel. Spontaneous decay of response-code activation , 1994, Psychological research.
[23] M. Botvinick,et al. Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. , 2001, Psychological review.
[24] N. Meiran,et al. On the origins of the task mixing cost in the cuing task-switching paradigm. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[25] N. Meiran. Task Rule-Congruency and Simon-Like Effects in Switching between Spatial Tasks , 2005, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.
[26] N. Meiran. Modeling cognitive control in task-switching , 2000, Psychological research.
[27] R De Jong,et al. An intention-activation account of residual switch costs , 2000 .
[28] R. Kliegl,et al. Task-set switching and long-term memory retrieval. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[29] Darryl W. Schneider. Isolating a mediated route for response congruency effects in task switching. , 2015, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[30] D. Alan Allport,et al. SHIFTING INTENTIONAL SET - EXPLORING THE DYNAMIC CONTROL OF TASKS , 1994 .
[31] The target of task switching. , 2010, Canadian journal of experimental psychology = Revue canadienne de psychologie experimentale.
[32] J. Driver,et al. Control of Cognitive Processes: Attention and Performance XVIII , 2000 .
[33] Yoav Kessler,et al. Control by action representation and input selection (CARIS): a theoretical framework for task switching , 2008, Psychological research.
[34] Rethinking the role of automaticity in cognitive control , 2012, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[35] G. Logan,et al. When it helps to be misled: Facilitative effects of increasing the frequency of conflicting stimuli in a Stroop-like task , 1979 .
[36] L. Jacoby,et al. Why it is too early to lose control in accounts of item-specific proportion congruency effects. , 2011, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[37] James R. Schmidt,et al. List-level transfer effects in temporal learning: Further complications for the list-level proportion congruent effect , 2014 .
[38] Keith A Hutchison,et al. Converging evidence for control of color-word Stroop interference at the item level. , 2013, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[39] Gordon D. Logan,et al. Strategies in the color-word Stroop task , 1984 .
[40] E. Donchin,et al. Optimizing the use of information: strategic control of activation of responses. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[41] Mike Wendt,et al. The impact of stimulus-specific practice and task instructions on response congruency effects between tasks , 2008, Psychological research.
[42] Todd S. Braver,et al. A model of dual control mechanisms through anterior cingulate and prefrontal cortex interactions , 2006, Neurocomputing.
[43] Jeffrey P. Toth,et al. Dissociation of Processes Underlying Spatial S-R Compatibility: Evidence for the Independent Influence of What and Where , 1995, Consciousness and Cognition.
[44] D. Algom,et al. Driven by information: a tectonic theory of Stroop effects. , 2003, Psychological review.
[45] Darryl W. Schneider,et al. Modeling task switching without switching tasks: a short-term priming account of explicitly cued performance. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[46] J. Stroop. Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. , 1992 .
[47] Emily R. Cohen-Shikora,et al. Expectations and experience: Dissociable bases for cognitive control? , 2015, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[48] Matthew J. C. Crump,et al. In Support of a Distinction between Voluntary and Stimulus-Driven Control: A Review of the Literature on Proportion Congruent Effects , 2012, Front. Psychology.
[49] Andrea Kiesel,et al. Top-down versus bottom-up: when instructions overcome automatic retrieval , 2013, Psychological research.
[50] Chris Blais,et al. Behavioral and Neural Evidence for Item-specific Performance Monitoring , 2010, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[51] D. A. Taylor,et al. The cuing and priming of cognitive operations. , 1987, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[52] N. Meiran,et al. Conflict control in task conflict and response conflict , 2014, Psychological Research.
[53] D. Lowe,et al. Selective and divided Attention in a Stroop task. , 1982, Canadian journal of psychology.
[54] James R. Schmidt,et al. The Stroop effect: why proportion congruent has nothing to do with congruency and everything to do with contingency. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[55] Stephen Monsell,et al. Residual costs in task switching: Testing the failure-to-engage hypothesis , 2002, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[56] Robert W. Proctor,et al. Automaticity without extensive training: The role of memory retrieval in implementation of task-defined rules , 2011, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[57] J. Bugg,et al. Conflict-triggered top-down control: default mode, last resort, or no such thing? , 2014, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[58] J. Algina,et al. Generalized eta and omega squared statistics: measures of effect size for some common research designs. , 2003, Psychological methods.
[59] A. Kiesel,et al. Task switching: on the origin of response congruency effects , 2007, Psychological research.
[60] Andrea M Philipp,et al. The role of inhibition in task switching: A review , 2010, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[61] J. Bugg,et al. List-wide control is not entirely elusive: Evidence from picture–word Stroop , 2011, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[62] A. Vandierendonck,et al. Task switching: interplay of reconfiguration and interference control. , 2010, Psychological bulletin.
[63] Colin M. Macleod. Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: an integrative review. , 1991, Psychological bulletin.
[64] B. Hommel,et al. Task-switching and long-term priming: Role of episodic stimulus–task bindings in task-shift costs , 2003, Cognitive Psychology.
[65] T. Egner. Congruency sequence effects and cognitive control , 2007, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.
[66] Derek Besner,et al. Item-specific adaptation and the conflict-monitoring hypothesis: a computational model. , 2007, Psychological review.
[67] S. Hsieh,et al. Resolving task rule incongruence during task switching by competitor rule suppression. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[68] K. Hutchison,et al. The Interactive Effects of Listwide Control, Item-based Control, and Working Memory Capacity on Stroop Performance and Holly Vander Vos for Scheduling and Running Participants for This Study. I Am Also Grateful To , 2022 .
[69] Mike Wendt,et al. Conflict-frequency affects flanker interference: role of stimulus-ensemble-specific practiceand flanker-response contingencies. , 2009, Experimental psychology.
[70] Daniel Algom,et al. The stroop effect: It is not the robust phenomenon that you have thought it to be , 2000, Memory & cognition.
[71] L. Jacoby,et al. Multiple levels of control in the Stroop task , 2008, Memory & cognition.
[72] Kimberly S. Chiew,et al. Temporal Dynamics of Motivation-Cognitive Control Interactions Revealed by High-Resolution Pupillometry , 2013, Front. Psychology.
[73] F. Karayanidis,et al. The many faces of preparatory control in task switching: Reviewing a decade of fMRI research , 2013, Human brain mapping.
[74] W. Notebaert,et al. Hebbian learning of cognitive control: dealing with specific and nonspecific adaptation. , 2008, Psychological review.
[75] L. Jacoby,et al. Stroop process dissociations: the relationship between facilitation and interference. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[76] N. Meiran. Reconfiguration of processing mode prior to task performance. , 1996 .
[77] Mary Thomas. You Can't Have It Both Ways: An Examination of Congruency Effects in Task Switching. , 2010 .
[78] M. McDaniel,et al. Revealing list-level control in the Stroop task by uncovering its benefits and a cost. , 2011, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.