Depth information affects judgment of the surface-color mode appearance.
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] K. Uchikawa,et al. Upper-limit luminance for the surface-color mode appearance. , 2000, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.
[2] L. Arend,et al. Perceived lightness, but not brightness, of achromatic surfaces depends on perceived depth information , 1990, Perception & psychophysics.
[3] S. Shevell,et al. Lightness and brightness judgments of coplanar retinally noncontiguous surfaces. , 1993, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.
[4] D. Macleod,et al. Color appearance depends on the variance of surround colors , 1997, Current Biology.
[5] A. Gilchrist. Perceived lightness depends on perceived spatial arrangement. , 1977, Science.
[6] Ralph M. Evans. Fluorescence and Gray Content of Surface Colors , 1959 .
[7] Steven K Shevell,et al. Chromatic induction: border contrast or adaptation to surrounding light? , 1998, Vision Research.
[8] A. Gilchrist,et al. Perceived area and the luminosity threshold , 1999, Perception & psychophysics.
[9] L. Arend,et al. Simultaneous constancy, lightness, and brightness. , 1987, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.
[10] F. Bonato,et al. The effects of figure/ground, perceived area, and target saliency on the luminosity threshold , 2000, Perception & psychophysics.
[11] D R Proffitt,et al. Perceptual Organization Evokes Simultaneous Lightness Contrast , 1993, Perception.
[12] R. M. Evans,et al. Chromatic strength of colors: dominant wavelength and purity. , 1967, Journal of the Optical Society of America.
[13] J. Schirillo,et al. Three-dimensional spatial grouping affects estimates of the illuminant. , 2003, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.
[14] I. Kuriki,et al. Brightness, not luminance, determines transition from the surface-color to the aperture-color mode for colored lights , 2001 .
[15] Hiroyuki Shinoda,et al. Three Dimensionality of the Recognized Visual Space of Illumination Proved by Hidden Illumination , 1998 .
[16] Alan Gilchrist,et al. Anchoring of lightness values in center-surround displays. , 1995 .
[17] G. Buchsbaum,et al. The effect of spatial and chromatic parameters on chromatic induction , 1988 .
[18] H. Wallach. Brightness constancy and the nature of achromatic colors. , 1948, Journal of experimental psychology.
[19] L. Maloney,et al. The effect of perceived surface orientation on perceived surface albedo in binocularly viewed scenes. , 2003, Journal of vision.
[20] Keiji Uchikawa,et al. Limit of the Surface-Color Mode Perception under Non-Uniform Illuminations , 2004 .
[21] A. Gilchrist,et al. The Perception of Luminosity on Different Backgrounds and in Different Illuminations , 1994, Perception.
[22] Robin E. Hauck,et al. Measurements of the effect of surface slant on perceived lightness. , 2004, Journal of vision.
[23] P. Vernon. The World of Colour , 1935 .
[24] A. Gilchrist,et al. An anchoring theory of lightness perception. , 1999, Psychological review.
[25] D. Brainard,et al. Luminosity thresholds: effects of test chromaticity and ambient illumination. , 1996, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.
[26] E. Adelson. Perceptual organization and the judgment of brightness. , 1993, Science.
[27] A. Gilchrist,et al. Local and global processes in surface lightness perception , 1995, Perception & psychophysics.