Decline in motor prediction in elderly subjects: Right versus left arm differences in mentally simulated motor actions

This study investigates the effects of age upon the temporal features of executed and imagined movements performed with the dominant (D; right) and nondominant (ND; left) arms. Thirty right-handed subjects were divided into two groups: (i) the young group (n=15; mean age: 22.5+/-2.5 years) and (ii) the elderly group (n=15; mean age: 70.2+/-2.2 years). The motor task, involving arm pointing movements among four pairs of targets (.5cm, 1cm, 1.5cm and 2cm), imposed strong spatiotemporal constraints. During overt performance, young and elderly subjects modulated movement duration according to the size of targets, despite the fact that movement speed decreased with age as well as in the left arm compared with the right. This observation was also valid for the covert performance produced by the young group. However, such a strong relationship between covert movement durations and target size was not as obvious in the elderly group. Young, compared to elderly subjects, showed stronger correlations and smaller absolute differences between executed and imagined movements for both arms. Additionally, the absolute difference between executed and imagined arm movement durations was more pronounced for the left than the right arm in aged subjects. This result suggests a selective decline with age of mental prediction of motor actions, which is more prominent when the ND arm is involved.

[1]  J L Bradshaw,et al.  Response (re-)programming in aging: a kinematic analysis. , 1998, The journals of gerontology. Series A, Biological sciences and medical sciences.

[2]  Marco Schieppati,et al.  Does order and timing in performance of imagined and actual movements affect the motor imagery process? The duration of walking and writing task , 2002, Behavioural Brain Research.

[3]  A. Sirigu,et al.  The Mental Representation of Hand Movements After Parietal Cortex Damage , 1996, Science.

[4]  Alain Berthoz,et al.  Comparison of actual and imagined execution of whole-body movements after a long exposure to microgravity , 2003, Neuroscience Letters.

[5]  T. Pozzo,et al.  Improvement and generalization of arm motor performance through motor imagery practice , 2006, Neuroscience.

[6]  Daniel M. Wolpert,et al.  Forward Models for Physiological Motor Control , 1996, Neural Networks.

[7]  J. Logan,et al.  Under-Recruitment and Nonselective Recruitment Dissociable Neural Mechanisms Associated with Aging , 2002, Neuron.

[8]  F. Schmitt,et al.  Critical decline in fine motor hand movements in human aging. , 1999, Neurology.

[9]  L. Craighero,et al.  Electrophysiology of Action Representation , 2004, Journal of clinical neurophysiology : official publication of the American Electroencephalographic Society.

[10]  S. Swinnen,et al.  The effect of aging on dynamic position sense at the ankle , 2002, Behavioural Brain Research.

[11]  Magdalena Sabaté,et al.  Brain lateralization of motor imagery: motor planning asymmetry as a cause of movement lateralization , 2004, Neuropsychologia.

[12]  Michael Alexander,et al.  Age-Related Differences in Movement Representation , 2002, NeuroImage.

[13]  T. Salthouse Aging and measures of processing speed , 2000, Biological Psychology.

[14]  G. Courtine,et al.  Gait-dependent motor memory facilitation in covert movement execution. , 2004, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[15]  Susanne Ferber,et al.  Selective, Non-lateralized Impairment of Motor Imagery Following Right Parietal Damage , 2002, Neurocase.

[16]  G. Stelmach,et al.  Changes in multi-joint performance with age. , 2002, Motor control.

[17]  N. Raz,et al.  Age-related deficits in generation and manipulation of mental images: I. The role of sensorimotor speed and working memory. , 1999, Psychology and aging.

[18]  P. Reuter-Lorenz New visions of the aging mind and brain , 2002, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[19]  S. Swinnen,et al.  Neural Basis of Aging: The Penetration of Cognition into Action Control , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[20]  M. D’Esposito,et al.  Isolating the neural mechanisms of age-related changes in human working memory , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[21]  G. Stelmach,et al.  Persistence in visual feedback control by the elderly , 1998, Experimental Brain Research.

[22]  Cindy Lustig,et al.  Brain aging: reorganizing discoveries about the aging mind , 2005, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[23]  R. Gentili,et al.  Inertial properties of the arm are accurately predicted during motor imagery , 2004, Behavioural Brain Research.

[24]  M. Jeannerod,et al.  The timing of mentally represented actions , 1989, Behavioural Brain Research.

[25]  J. Decety The neurophysiological basis of motor imagery , 1996, Behavioural Brain Research.

[26]  D. Wolpert,et al.  Motor prediction , 2001, Current Biology.

[27]  J. Callicott,et al.  Neurophysiological correlates of age-related changes in human motor function , 2002, Neurology.

[28]  C. Rice,et al.  Normalized force, activation, and coactivation in the arm muscles of young and old men. , 2001, Journal of applied physiology.

[29]  T. Pozzo,et al.  Mentally represented motor actions in normal aging I. Age effects on the temporal features of overt and covert execution of actions , 2005, Behavioural Brain Research.

[30]  S. Kosslyn,et al.  Mental imagery and aging. , 1994, Psychology and aging.

[31]  C. Maganaris,et al.  Effect of aging on human muscle architecture. , 2003, Journal of applied physiology.

[32]  N. Raz,et al.  Age-related deficits in generation and manipulation of mental images: II. The role of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. , 1999, Psychology and aging.

[33]  M. Jeannerod Neural Simulation of Action: A Unifying Mechanism for Motor Cognition , 2001, NeuroImage.

[34]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  Age-related changes in the neural correlates of motor performance. , 2003, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[35]  Marco Schieppati,et al.  Imagined and actual arm movements have similar durations when performed under different conditions of direction and mass , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.

[36]  M. Sale,et al.  Age-related differences in corticospinal control during functional isometric contractions in left and right hands. , 2005, Journal of applied physiology.

[37]  D. Sale,et al.  A 12-year follow-up study of ankle muscle function in older adults. , 1996, The journals of gerontology. Series A, Biological sciences and medical sciences.

[38]  R. Sainburg Evidence for a dynamic-dominance hypothesis of handedness , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.

[39]  George E Stelmach,et al.  Age-related kinematic differences as influenced by task difficulty, target size, and movement amplitude. , 2002, The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences.

[40]  R. C. Oldfield The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.

[41]  P. Maruff,et al.  Asymmetries between dominant and non-dominanthands in real and imagined motor task performance , 1999, Neuropsychologia.