The impact of Parkinson’s disease on vocal-prosodic communication from the perspective of listeners

An expressive disturbance of speech prosody has long been associated with idiopathic Parkinson's disease (PD), but little is known about the impact of dysprosody on vocal-prosodic communication from the perspective of listeners. Recordings of healthy adults (n=12) and adults with mild to moderate PD (n=21) were elicited in four speech contexts in which prosody serves a primary function in linguistic or emotive communication (phonemic stress, contrastive stress, sentence mode, and emotional prosody). Twenty independent listeners naive to the disease status of individual speakers then judged the intended meanings conveyed by prosody for tokens recorded in each condition. Findings indicated that PD speakers were less successful at communicating stress distinctions, especially words produced with contrastive stress, which were identifiable to listeners. Listeners were also significantly less able to detect intended emotional qualities of Parkinsonian speech, especially for anger and disgust. Emotional expressions that were correctly recognized by listeners were consistently rated as less intense for the PD group. Utterances produced by PD speakers were frequently characterized as sounding sad or devoid of emotion entirely (neutral). Results argue that motor limitations on the vocal apparatus in PD produce serious and early negative repercussions on communication through prosody, which diminish the social-linguistic competence of Parkinsonian adults as judged by listeners.

[1]  M. Pell On the Receptive Prosodic Loss in Parkinson's Disease , 1996, Cortex.

[2]  K. Scherer,et al.  Acoustic profiles in vocal emotion expression. , 1996, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[3]  Philip Lieberman,et al.  Intonation, Perception and Language , 1968 .

[4]  A. Aronson,et al.  Motor Speech Disorders , 2014 .

[5]  M. Pell,et al.  Processing emotional tone from speech in Parkinson’s disease: A role for the basal ganglia , 2003, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[6]  G. Canter SPEECH CHARACTERISTICS OF PATIENTS WITH PARKINSON'S DISEASE. 3. ARTICULATION, DIADOCHOKINESIS, AND OVER-ALL SPEECH ADEQUACY. , 1965, The Journal of speech and hearing disorders.

[7]  Raymond D. Kent,et al.  Prosodic disturbance and neurologic lesion , 1982, Brain and Language.

[8]  Howard Poizner,et al.  Articulatory Consequences of Parkinson's Disease: Perspectives from Two Modalities , 1999, Brain and Cognition.

[9]  M. Van der Linden,et al.  The interpretation of dysprosody in patients with Parkinson's disease. , 1991, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[10]  K. Stevens,et al.  Emotions and speech: some acoustical correlates. , 1972, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[11]  William E. Cooper,et al.  Fundamental Frequency in Sentence Production , 1981 .

[12]  P. Laukka,et al.  Communication of emotions in vocal expression and music performance: different channels, same code? , 2003, Psychological bulletin.

[13]  J. Pierrehumbert The perception of fundamental frequency declination. , 1979, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[14]  A. Lloyd,et al.  Comprehension of Prosody in Parkinson's Disease , 1999, Cortex.

[15]  M. Pell Influence of emotion and focus location on prosody in matched statements and questions. , 2001, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[16]  I. Daum,et al.  Impaired Perception of Vocal Emotions in Parkinson's Disease: Influence of Speech Time Processing and Executive Functioning , 2001, Brain and Cognition.

[17]  Daniel Kempler,et al.  Effect of Speech Task on Intelligibility in Dysarthria: A Case Study of Parkinson's Disease , 2002, Brain and Language.

[18]  M Studdert-Kennedy,et al.  Auditory and Linguistic Processes in the Perception of Intonation Contours , 1973, Language and speech.

[19]  Gwenn W. Gröndal,et al.  Meta-analytic procedures for social research , 1993 .

[20]  A. Flint,et al.  Abnormal speech articulation, psychomotor retardation, and subcortical dysfunction in major depression. , 1993, Journal of psychiatric research.

[21]  D. V. von Cramon,et al.  Syntactic comprehension in Parkinson's disease: investigating early automatic and late integrational processes using event-related brain potentials. , 2003, Neuropsychology.

[22]  E. Critchley,et al.  Speech disorders of Parkinsonism: a review. , 1981, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[23]  T. Benke,et al.  A Study of Emotional Processing in Parkinson's Disease , 1998, Brain and Cognition.

[24]  J. Darby,et al.  Speech and voice parameters of depression: a pilot study. , 1984, Journal of communication disorders.

[25]  G. Canter SPEECH CHARACTERISTICS OF PATIENTS WITH PARKINSON'S DISEASE. II. PHYSIOLOGICAL SUPPORT FOR SPEECH. , 1965, The Journal of speech and hearing disorders.

[26]  D. Klatt Linguistic uses of segmental duration in English: acoustic and perceptual evidence. , 1976, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[27]  R Blasdell,et al.  Influence of fundamental frequency cues on the perception of some synthetic intonation contours. , 1969, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[28]  P. Snyder,et al.  Acoustic characteristics of Parkinsonian speech: a potential biomarker of early disease progression and treatment , 2004, Journal of Neurolinguistics.

[29]  V. Fromkin,et al.  A characterization of the prosodic loss in Parkinson's disease , 1988, Brain and Language.

[30]  R. Collier,et al.  Declination: Construct or Intrinsic Feature of Speech Pitch ? , 1982, Phonetica.

[31]  J. Turner Human Emotions: A Sociological Theory , 2007 .

[32]  B. Snow,et al.  Criteria for diagnosing Parkinson's disease , 1992, Annals of neurology.

[33]  Murray Grossman,et al.  Information processing speed and sentence comprehension in Parkinson's disease. , 2002, Neuropsychology.

[34]  M. Bodt,et al.  Intelligibility as a linear combination of dimensions in dysarthric speech. , 2002 .

[35]  J. Ryalls,et al.  The effects of speech and language therapy for a case of dysarthria associated with Parkinson's disease. , 1992, European journal of disorders of communication : the journal of the College of Speech and Language Therapists, London.

[36]  D. Benson,et al.  Alzheimer's disease and Parkinson's disease , 1988, Neurology.

[37]  G. Canter SPEECH CHARACTERISTICS OF PATIENTS WITH PARKINSON'S DISEASE: I. INTENSITY, PITCH, AND DURATION. , 1963, The Journal of speech and hearing disorders.

[38]  M. Pell,et al.  Facial expression decoding in early Parkinson's disease. , 2005, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[39]  M. Pell Evaluation of Nonverbal Emotion in Face and Voice: Some Preliminary Findings on a New Battery of Tests , 2002, Brain and Cognition.

[40]  C A Fowler,et al.  Fundamental frequency declination is not unique to human speech: evidence from nonhuman primates. , 1992, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[41]  R. Gur,et al.  The effects of right and left hemiparkinsonism on prosody , 1989, Brain and Language.

[42]  J. Illes Neurolinguistic features of spontaneous language production dissociate three forms of neurodegenerative disease: Alzheimer's, Huntington's, and Parkinson's , 1989, Brain and Language.

[43]  H H Stassen,et al.  Speaking behavior and voice sound characteristics in depressive patients during recovery. , 1993, Journal of psychiatric research.

[44]  H. Ackermann,et al.  Dysprosody in Parkinson's disease: an investigation of intonation patterns , 2001 .

[45]  B Pentland,et al.  Impressions of parkinsonian patients from their recorded voices. , 1990, The British journal of disorders of communication.

[46]  Information processing speed and sentence comprehension in Parkinson's disease. , 2002 .

[47]  Lena Hartelius,et al.  High-level language difficulties in Parkinson's disease , 2003, Clinical linguistics & phonetics.

[48]  Patrick McNamara,et al.  Pragmatic communication skills in patients with Parkinson’s disease , 2003, Brain and Language.

[49]  F. Caird,et al.  Evidence for an apparent sensory speech disorder in Parkinson's disease. , 1984, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.