The Control and Perception of Finger Forces

The human hand demonstrates remarkable dexterity in its capacity to control precisely the forces involved in manipulating objects and the timing of movements during the execution of skilled motor tasks. In all of these endeavors, mechanoreceptors in the skin play a critical role in encoding the timing, magnitude, direction and spatial distribution of fingertip forces. When cutaneous inputs are absent or deficient, the hand is unable to compensate rapidly when an object begins to slip between the digits, and misdirected finger movements are not recognized and corrected. The control and perception of forces generated by the hand therefore relies on a close interplay between the sensory and motor systems. When sensory information changes, the capacity to control and modulate force can be disrupted and this in turn influences the perceived magnitude of the forces being produced. Cutaneous mechanoreceptors provide crucial information about the forces produced by the fingers and these inputs together with centrally generated corollary discharges are fundamental to the human perception of force.

[1]  L. A. Jones Matching Forces: Constant Errors and Differential Thresholds , 1989, Perception.

[2]  H. Kinoshita,et al.  Contributions and co-ordination of individual fingers in multiple finger prehension. , 1995, Ergonomics.

[3]  K. Connolly,et al.  The psychobiology of the hand , 1998 .

[4]  S. Bolanowski,et al.  Four channels mediate the mechanical aspects of touch. , 1988, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[5]  Bernard J. Martin,et al.  Keyboard Reaction Force and Finger Flexor Electromyograms during Computer Keyboard Work , 1996, Hum. Factors.

[6]  C. E. Chapman,et al.  Role of friction and tangential force variation in the subjective scaling of tactile roughness , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.

[7]  S C Gandevia,et al.  Neural and biomechanical specializations of human thumb muscles revealed by matching weights and grasping objects. , 1993, The Journal of physiology.

[8]  A. M. Smith,et al.  Subjective scaling of smooth surface friction. , 1996, Journal of neurophysiology.

[9]  J C Stevens,et al.  How skin and object temperature influence touch sensation , 1982, Perception & psychophysics.

[10]  Mandayam A. Srinivasan,et al.  Surface Microgeometry: Tactile Perception and Neural Encoding , 1991 .

[11]  Lynette A. Jones,et al.  Material discrimination and thermal perception , 2003, 11th Symposium on Haptic Interfaces for Virtual Environment and Teleoperator Systems, 2003. HAPTICS 2003. Proceedings..

[12]  J. Randall Flanagan,et al.  Coding and use of tactile signals from the fingertips in object manipulation tasks , 2009, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[13]  V. Mathiowetz,et al.  Grip and pinch strength: normative data for adults. , 1985, Archives of physical medicine and rehabilitation.

[14]  Antony W Goodwin,et al.  Human Ability to Scale and Discriminate Forces Typical of Those Occurring during Grasp and Manipulation , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[15]  S. Lederman,et al.  Human Hand Function , 2006 .

[16]  H. Ross,et al.  Sensorimotor mechanisms in weight discrimination , 1984, Perception & psychophysics.

[17]  K. Reilly,et al.  Incomplete functional subdivision of the human multitendoned finger muscle flexor digitorum profundus: an electromyographic study. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[18]  M. Srinivasan,et al.  Tactile detection of slip: surface microgeometry and peripheral neural codes. , 1990, Journal of neurophysiology.

[19]  L. A. Jones,et al.  The senses of effort and force during fatiguing contractions. , 1995, Advances in experimental medicine and biology.

[20]  J. Hermsdörfer,et al.  Changes in perceived finger force produced by muscular contractions under isometric and anisometric conditions , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[21]  S. Gandevia,et al.  Differential control of the digits of the human hand: evidence from digital anaesthesia and weight matching , 1997, Experimental Brain Research.

[22]  Astrid M. L. Kappers,et al.  Cues for Haptic Perception of Compliance , 2009, IEEE Transactions on Haptics.

[23]  Christine L. MacKenzie,et al.  The Grasping Hand , 2011, The Grasping Hand.

[24]  B J Martin,et al.  The effect of keyboard keyswitch make force on applied force and finger flexor muscle activity. , 1997, Ergonomics.

[25]  Charlotte Wolff The Human Hand , 2015 .

[26]  Ove Franzén,et al.  Information Processing in the Somatosensory System , 1991 .

[27]  Mark R. Cutkosky,et al.  Modeling manufacturing grips and correlations with the design of robotic hands , 1986, Proceedings. 1986 IEEE International Conference on Robotics and Automation.

[28]  I. Hunter,et al.  A perceptual analysis of viscosity , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[29]  Lynette A. Jones,et al.  Perceptual constancy and the perceived magnitude of muscle forces , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[30]  C. Palmer Mapping musical thought to musical performance. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[31]  C. E. Chapman,et al.  Relative effects of the spatial and temporal characteristics of scanned surfaces on human perception of tactile roughness using passive touch , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.

[32]  M A Srinivasan,et al.  Manual discrimination of compliance using active pinch grasp: The roles of force and work cues , 1995, Perception & psychophysics.

[33]  J. T. Dennerlein,et al.  Control strategies for finger movement during touch-typing The role of the extrinsic muscles during a keystroke , 1998, Experimental Brain Research.

[34]  R. Johansson,et al.  Signals in tactile afferents from the fingers eliciting adaptive motor responses during precision grip , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[35]  L. Jones,et al.  Contribution of tactile feedback from the hand to the perception of force , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.

[36]  Astrid M. L. Kappers,et al.  Haptic Perception of Viscosity , 2010, EuroHaptics.

[37]  I Hunter,et al.  Viscosity Discrimination: A Comparison of an Adaptive Two-Alternative Forced-Choice and an Adjustment Procedure , 1997, Perception.

[38]  D. Burke,et al.  Perceptual responses to microstimulation of single afferents innervating joints, muscles and skin of the human hand. , 1990, The Journal of physiology.

[39]  S C Gandevia,et al.  Independent digit control: failure to partition perceived heaviness of weights lifted by digits of the human hand. , 1991, The Journal of physiology.

[40]  J. F. Soechting,et al.  Use of tactile afferent information in sequential finger movements , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[41]  G. Loeb,et al.  Mathematical models of proprioceptors. II. Structure and function of the Golgi tendon organ. , 2006, Journal of neurophysiology.

[42]  Andrew M. Gordon,et al.  Initiation and development of fingertip forces during whole-hand grasping , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.

[43]  Lynette A. Jones,et al.  Visual and haptic feedback in the control of force , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[44]  Hong Z. Tan,et al.  HUMAN FACTORS FOR THE DESIGN OF FORCE-REFLECTING HAPTIC INTERFACES , 1994 .

[45]  Hsin-Ni Ho,et al.  Modeling the thermal responses of the skin surface during hand-object interactions. , 2008, Journal of biomechanical engineering.

[46]  Norbert Mai,et al.  Maintenance of low isometric forces during prehensile grasping , 1985, Neuropsychologia.

[47]  R. Johansson,et al.  Independent control of human finger‐tip forces at individual digits during precision lifting. , 1992, The Journal of physiology.

[48]  I. Hunter,et al.  A perceptual analysis of stiffness , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[49]  Marc H Schieber,et al.  Hand function: peripheral and central constraints on performance. , 2004, Journal of applied physiology.

[50]  K. Reilly,et al.  Independence of force production by digits of the human hand , 2000, Neuroscience Letters.

[51]  S. Gandevia,et al.  Accuracy of weight estimation for weights lifted by proximal and distal muscles of the human upper limb. , 1990, The Journal of physiology.

[52]  Simon C. Gandevia,et al.  Fatigue : neural and muscular mechanisms , 1995 .

[53]  Lynette A. Jones,et al.  Thermal cues and the perception of force , 2009, Experimental Brain Research.

[54]  T Ohtsuki,et al.  Decrease in grip strength induced by simultaneous bilateral exertion with reference to finger strength. , 1981, Ergonomics.

[55]  N. Durlach,et al.  Manual discrimination of force using active finger motion , 1991, Perception & psychophysics.

[56]  K. A. Ericsson,et al.  Maintaining excellence: deliberate practice and elite performance in young and older pianists. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[57]  L. Jami Golgi tendon organs in mammalian skeletal muscle: functional properties and central actions. , 1992, Physiological reviews.