A quantitative measurement of binocular color fusion limit for different disparities

Color asymmetry is a common phenomenon in stereoscopic display system, which can cause visual fatigue or visual discomfort. When the color difference between the left and right eyes exceeds a threshold value, named binocular color fusion limit, color rivalry is said to occur. The most important information brought by stereoscopic displays is the depth perception produced by the disparity. As the stereo pair stimuli are presented separately to both eyes with disparities and those two monocular stimuli differ in color but share an iso-luminance polarity, it is possible for stereopsis and color rivalry to coexist. In this paper, we conducted an experiment to measure the color fusion limit for different disparity levels. In particular, it examines how the magnitude and sign of disparity affect the binocular color fusion limit that yields a fused, stable stereoscopic percept. The binocular color fusion limit was measured at five levels of disparities: 0, ±60, ±120 arc minutes for a sample color point which was selected from the 1976 CIE u'v' chromaticity diagram. The experimental results showed that fusion limit for the sample point varied with the level and sign of disparity. It was an interesting result that the fusion limit increased as the disparity decreases at crossed disparity direction (sign −), but there is almost no big change at uncrossed disparity direction (sign +). We found that color fusion was more difficult to achieve at the crossed disparity direction than at the uncrossed disparity direction.

[1]  D JAMESON,et al.  The binocular fusion of yellow in relation to color theories. , 1951, Science.

[2]  Yoshio Nakashima,et al.  Wavelength difference limit for binocular color fusion , 1980, Vision Research.

[3]  M Ikeda,et al.  Binocular color fusion limit. , 1979, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[4]  Yoshio Nakashima,et al.  Effects of luminance and size of stimuli upon binocular color fusion limit , 2009 .

[5]  Yoshio Nakashima,et al.  Research of Binocular Colour Fusion Limit on Peripheral Visual Field , 2007 .

[6]  Wijnand A. IJsselsteijn,et al.  A survey of perceptual evaluations and requirements of three-dimensional TV , 2004, IEEE Transactions on Circuits and Systems for Video Technology.

[7]  R. W. PICKFORD Binocular Colour Combinations , 1947, Nature.

[8]  Dorothea E. Johannsen,et al.  A Quantitative Study of Binocular Color Vision , 1930 .

[9]  Zaiqing Chen,et al.  Visual discomfort caused by color asymmetry in 3D displays , 2016, SPIE/COS Photonics Asia.

[10]  Zaiqing Chen,et al.  A design of near-eye 3D display based on dual-OLED , 2010, Other Conferences.

[11]  Mtm Marc Lambooij,et al.  Visual Discomfort and Visual Fatigue of Stereoscopic Displays: A Review , 2009 .

[12]  S. Eckstein Ethical principles for medical research involving human subjects. , 2001, European journal of emergency medicine : official journal of the European Society for Emergency Medicine.

[13]  N. Wade,et al.  The influence of colour and contour rivalry on the magnitude of the tilt after-effect , 1978, Vision Research.

[14]  I. Kovács,et al.  When the brain changes its mind: interocular grouping during binocular rivalry. , 1996, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[15]  Yong Man Ro,et al.  Quantitative measurement of binocular color fusion limit for non-spectral colors. , 2011, Optics express.

[16]  Sam Y. Bae,et al.  Single lens dual-aperture 3D imaging system: color modeling , 2012, Electronic Imaging.

[17]  Alexander Toet,et al.  Visual comfort of binocular and 3D displays , 2004 .

[18]  J K Hovis Review of dichoptic color mixing. , 1989, Optometry and vision science : official publication of the American Academy of Optometry.

[19]  Yoshio Nakashima,et al.  Change of Wavelength Difference Limit for Binocular Color Fusion with Wavelength and Brightness of Stimuli , 2006 .