Shared means and meanings in vocal expression of man and macaque

Vocalisations of six Macaca arctoides that were categorised according to their social context and judgements of naïve human listeners as expressions of plea/submission, anger, fear, dominance, contentment and emotional neutrality, were compared with vowel samples extracted from simulations of emotional-motivational connotations by the Finnish name Saara and English name Sarah. The words were spoken by seven Finnish and 13 English women. Humans and monkeys resembled each other in the following respects. 'Neutral', 'pleading' and 'commanding' had a similar F0 level. Loud vocalisations of intense 'anger' and 'fear' had both high F0, and the highest values were encountered for 'fear'. Compared with 'neutral', the audiosignal waveform of 'plea/submission' was more sinusoidal, seen in the spectrum as an attenuation of formants and an emphasis of the fundamental, whereas the signal waveform of 'commanding' was more complex corresponding to an increase in noise and a wider distribution of spectral energy. 'Frightened' samples included rather harmonic segments with emphasis of the fundamental, and 'angry' samples included more noise at the low end of the spectrum and often segments with low-frequency (<100 Hz) edged modulation. Sounds resembling soft and noisy 'content' grunts of the monkey do not appear in Finnish or English speech but 'content' utterances were, however, associated with low speech pressure, attenuation of harmonics and increase in noise.

[1]  R. Seyfarth,et al.  MALE GRUNTS AS MEDIATORS OF SOCIAL INTERACTION WITH FEMALES IN WILD CHACMA BABOONS (PAPIO CYNOCEPHALUS URSINUS) , 1999 .

[2]  Uwe Jürgens,et al.  Vocalization as an Emotional Indicator , 1979 .

[3]  K. Bard,et al.  Influences on development in infant chimpanzees: enculturation temperament, and cognition , 1996 .

[4]  W. L. Höffe,et al.  Über Beziehungen von Sprachmelodie und Lautstärke , 1960 .

[5]  M. Bornstein,et al.  Mothers’ attuned responses to infant affect expressivity promote earlier achievement of language milestones , 1999 .

[6]  J. Perkell,et al.  Glottal airflow and transglottal air pressure measurements for male and female speakers in soft, normal, and loud voice. , 1988, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[7]  B. Morrongiello,et al.  Crossmodal learning in newborn infants: Inferences about properties of auditory-visual events , 1998 .

[8]  N. Masataka,et al.  Pitch characteristics of Japanese maternal speech to infants , 1992, Journal of Child Language.

[9]  T. Hertz,et al.  Toward more comparability and generalizability of developmental research. , 1976 .

[10]  K. Eklund,et al.  Social interactional behaviors and symbolic play competence as predictors of language development and their associations with maternal attention-directing strategies , 1999 .

[11]  G. Fairbanks,et al.  An experimental study of the pitch characteristics of the voice during the expression of emotion , 1939 .

[12]  U. Jürgens,et al.  Vocalization as an emotional indicator. A neuroethological study in the squirrel monkey. , 1979, Behaviour.

[13]  I. Linnankoski,et al.  Recognition of emotions in macaque vocalizations by children and adults , 1994 .

[14]  H. Gouzoules,et al.  Representational Signaling in Non-Human Primate Vocal Communication , 1995 .

[15]  Uwe Jürgens,et al.  Neuronal Control of Vocal Production in Non-Human and Human Primates , 1995 .

[16]  Robert M. Seyfarth,et al.  Behavioral mechanisms underlying vocal communication in nonhuman primates , 1997 .

[17]  I. Linnankoski,et al.  Expression or emotional-motivational connotations with a one-word utterance. , 1997, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[18]  R. Wrangham,et al.  Does learning affect the structure of vocalizations in chimpanzees? , 1999, Animal Behaviour.

[19]  E. Kramer ELIMINATION OF VERBAL CUES IN JUDGMENTS OF EMOTION FROM VOICE. , 1964, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[20]  M. Bullowa Before Speech: The Beginning of Interpersonal Communication , 1979 .

[21]  D. Burnham,et al.  Universality and specificity in infant-directed speech: Pitch modifications as a function of infant age and sex in a tonal and non-tonal language , 2001 .

[22]  P. Alku,et al.  Physical variations related to stress and emotional state: A preliminary study. , 1996 .

[23]  W. Stebbins Hearing of old world monkeys (Cercopithecinae). , 1973, American journal of physical anthropology.

[24]  R. Feldman,et al.  Mother-infant affect synchrony as an antecedent of the emergence of self-control. , 1999, Developmental psychology.

[25]  K E Cummings,et al.  Analysis of the glottal excitation of emotionally styled and stressed speech. , 1995, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[26]  C. Fenster,et al.  Accuracy of vocal emotional communications among children and adults and the power of negative emotions. , 1977, Journal of communication disorders.

[27]  M M Vihman,et al.  Grunt communication in human infants (Homo sapiens). , 1996, Journal of comparative psychology.

[28]  Iain R. Murray,et al.  Toward the simulation of emotion in synthetic speech: a review of the literature on human vocal emotion. , 1993, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[29]  S. Green,et al.  Variation of Vocal Pattern with Social Situation in the Japanese Monkey (Macaca fuscata): A Field Study * , 1975 .

[30]  K. Scherer Nonlinguistic Vocal Indicators of Emotion and Psychopathology , 1979 .

[31]  R. Andrew,et al.  The origin and evolution of the calls and facial expressions of the primates. , 1963 .

[32]  J. Haviland,et al.  The Induced Affect Response: 10-Week-Old Infants' Responses to Three Emotion Expressions. , 1987 .

[33]  U. Jürgens The squirrel monkey as an experimental model in the study of cerebral organization of emotional vocal utterances , 2004, European archives of psychiatry and neurological sciences.

[34]  G. Kochanska,et al.  Implications of the mother-child relationship in infancy for socialization in the second year of life , 1999 .

[35]  D. Ploog,et al.  Vocal repertoire of the squirrel monkey (Saimiri sciureus), its analysis and significance , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[36]  C. Izard,et al.  Four- and six-month-old infants' visual responses to joy, anger, and neutral expressions. , 1976, Child development.

[37]  L Leinonen,et al.  Spectral pattern recognition of improved voice quality. , 1994, Journal of voice : official journal of the Voice Foundation.

[38]  K. Scherer,et al.  Acoustic profiles in vocal emotion expression. , 1996, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[39]  M. Bornstein,et al.  First words in the second year: Continuity, stability, and models of concurrent and predictive correspondence in vocabulary and verbal responsiveness across age and context , 1999 .

[40]  Uwe Jürgens,et al.  Current Topics in Primate Vocal Communication , 1995, Springer US.

[41]  S. Collins,et al.  Men's voices and women's choices , 2000, Animal Behaviour.

[42]  Some vocal distinctive features used by gelada monkeys. , 1976, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[43]  Cross-Cultural differences in the Perception of the Emotional Content of Speech: A Study of the Development of Sensitivity in Canadian and Mexican Children. , 1975 .

[44]  M J Owren,et al.  Vocalizations of rhesus (Macaca mulatta) and Japanese (M. fuscata) macaques cross-fostered between species show evidence of only limited modification. , 1993, Developmental psychobiology.

[45]  Renee van Bezooyen Characteristics and recognizability of vocal expressions of emotion , 1984 .

[46]  Raymond H. Colton,et al.  Elements of Voice Quality: Perceptual, Acoustic, and Physiologic Aspects , 1981 .

[47]  M. Neville The behavioral repertoire of the stumptail macaque. By Mireille Bertrand. xii + 273 pp. and 102 ill. Bibliotheca Primatologica, No. 11, S. Karger, Basel and New York. 1969. $14.65 , 1971 .

[48]  Effects of Stimulus Bandwidth on Listener Judgments of Vocal Loudness and Effort , 1972 .

[49]  Joseph S. Perkell,et al.  Erratum: ‘‘Glottal airflow and transglottal air pressure measurements for male and female speakers in soft, normal, and loud voice’’ [J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 84, 511–529 (1988)] , 1989 .

[50]  Reijo Aulanko,et al.  Vocal Communication between Species: Man and Macaque. , 1991 .

[51]  S. Chevalier-Skolnikoff,et al.  The ontogeny of communication in the stumptail macaque (Macaca arctiodes) , 1974 .

[52]  David Matsumoto,et al.  DEVELOPMENTAL CHARACTERISTICS IN JUDGMENTS OF EMOTION FROM NONVERBAL VOCAL CUES , 1983 .

[53]  U. Jürgens,et al.  The role of the periaqueductal grey in vocal behaviour , 1994, Behavioural Brain Research.

[54]  Marc H. Bornstein,et al.  Didactic interactions: Intuitive parental supports of vocal and verbal development in human infants , 1992 .

[55]  K. Scherer Expression of emotion in voice and music. , 1995, Journal of voice : official journal of the Voice Foundation.