Spatiotemporal Filter for Visual Motion Integration from Pursuit Eye Movements in Humans and Monkeys

Despite the enduring interest in motion integration, a direct measure of the space–time filter that the brain imposes on a visual scene has been elusive. This is perhaps because of the challenge of estimating a 3D function from perceptual reports in psychophysical tasks. We take a different approach. We exploit the close connection between visual motion estimates and smooth pursuit eye movements to measure stimulus–response correlations across space and time, computing the linear space–time filter for global motion direction in humans and monkeys. Although derived from eye movements, we find that the filter predicts perceptual motion estimates quite well. To distinguish visual from motor contributions to the temporal duration of the pursuit motion filter, we recorded single-unit responses in the monkey middle temporal cortical area (MT). We find that pursuit response delays are consistent with the distribution of cortical neuron latencies and that temporal motion integration for pursuit is consistent with a short integration MT subpopulation. Remarkably, the visual system appears to preferentially weight motion signals across a narrow range of foveal eccentricities rather than uniformly over the whole visual field, with a transiently enhanced contribution from locations along the direction of motion. We find that the visual system is most sensitive to motion falling at approximately one-third the radius of the stimulus aperture. Hypothesizing that the visual drive for pursuit is related to the filtered motion energy in a motion stimulus, we compare measured and predicted eye acceleration across several other target forms. SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT A compact model of the spatial and temporal processing underlying global motion perception has been elusive. We used visually driven smooth eye movements to find the 3D space–time function that best predicts both eye movements and perception of translating dot patterns. We found that the visual system does not appear to use all available motion signals uniformly, but rather weights motion preferentially in a narrow band at approximately one-third the radius of the stimulus. Although not universal, the filter predicts responses to other types of stimuli, demonstrating a remarkable degree of generalization that may lead to a deeper understanding of visual motion processing.

[1]  Jane E. Raymond,et al.  Directional anisotropy of motion sensitivity across the visual field , 1994, Vision Research.

[2]  F. A. Miles,et al.  The initial torsional Ocular Following Response (tOFR) in humans: a response to the total motion energy in the stimulus? , 2009, Journal of vision.

[3]  S. Lisberger,et al.  Representation of Perceptually Invisible Image Motion in Extrastriate Visual Area MT of Macaque Monkeys , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[4]  R. Born,et al.  Segregation of Object and Background Motion in Visual Area MT Effects of Microstimulation on Eye Movements , 2000, Neuron.

[5]  Randolph Blake,et al.  Contextual modulations of center-surround interactions in motion revealed with the motion aftereffect. , 2008, Journal of vision.

[6]  Scott N. J. Watamaniuk,et al.  Direction Perception in Complex Dynamic Displays: the Integration of Dir~~tion Information , 1988 .

[7]  B. Julesz,et al.  Displacement limits, directional anisotropy and direction versus form discrimination in random-dot cinematograms , 1983, Vision Research.

[8]  A. Watson,et al.  The optimal motion stimulus , 1995, Vision Research.

[9]  Alexander Grunewald,et al.  The Integration of Multiple Stimulus Features by V1 Neurons , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[10]  Duje Tadin,et al.  Optimal size for perceiving motion decreases with contrast , 2005, Vision Research.

[11]  D. Ringach,et al.  Dynamics of smooth pursuit maintenance. , 2009, Journal of neurophysiology.

[12]  Eileen Kowler,et al.  Sensitivity of smooth eye movement to small differences in target velocity , 1987, Vision Research.

[13]  Lawrence C. Sincich,et al.  Preserving Information in Neural Transmission , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[14]  Christopher C. Pack,et al.  Temporal dynamics of a neural solution to the aperture problem in visual area MT of macaque brain , 2001, Nature.

[15]  G. Orban,et al.  Comparative mapping of higher visual areas in monkeys and humans , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[16]  Shin'ya Nishida,et al.  Surround facilitation for rapid motion perception. , 2012, Journal of vision.

[17]  J B Mulligan,et al.  Sensory Processing Delays Measured with the Eye‐Movement Correlogram , 2002, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[18]  Maninder K. Kahlon,et al.  Visual Motion Analysis for Pursuit Eye Movements in Area MT of Macaque Monkeys , 1999, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[19]  L. Osborne,et al.  Shared Sensory Estimates for Human Motion Perception and Pursuit Eye Movements , 2015, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[20]  O. Braddick Segmentation versus integration in visual motion processing , 1993, Trends in Neurosciences.

[21]  D. Burr,et al.  Two stages of visual processing for radial and circular motion , 1995, Nature.

[22]  Visual pop-out in infancy: Effects of set-size on the latency of their eye movements , 2010 .

[23]  C. Rashbass,et al.  The relationship between saccadic and smooth tracking eye movements , 1961, The Journal of physiology.

[24]  W. Bialek,et al.  Time Course of Information about Motion Direction in Visual Area MT of Macaque Monkeys , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[25]  G. Orban,et al.  Human velocity and direction discrimination measured with random dot patterns , 1988, Vision Research.

[26]  Scott N. J. Watamaniuk,et al.  Temporal and spatial integration in dynamic random-dot stimuli , 1992, Vision Research.

[27]  K. H. Britten,et al.  A relationship between behavioral choice and the visual responses of neurons in macaque MT , 1996, Visual Neuroscience.

[28]  Patrick Cavanagh,et al.  Apparent speed increases at low luminance. , 2008, Journal of vision.

[29]  B. Batlogg,et al.  Auditory Spatial Receptive Fields Created by Multiplication , 2022 .

[30]  L. Paninski,et al.  Temporal Precision in the Visual Pathway through the Interplay of Excitation and Stimulus- Driven Suppression , 2022 .

[31]  R. Frostig,et al.  Cortical point-spread function and long-range lateral interactions revealed by real-time optical imaging of macaque monkey primary visual cortex , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[32]  W. Bialek,et al.  The Neural Basis for Combinatorial Coding in a Cortical Population Response , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[33]  W. Newsome,et al.  Deficits in visual motion processing following ibotenic acid lesions of the middle temporal visual area of the macaque monkey , 1985, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[34]  W. Bialek,et al.  A sensory source for motor variation , 2005, Nature.

[35]  G. Patricelli,et al.  Sexual selection: Male displays adjusted to female's response , 2002, Nature.

[36]  S. Dakin,et al.  The shape and size of crowding for moving targets , 2003, Vision Research.

[37]  Michael J. Black,et al.  Secrets of optical flow estimation and their principles , 2010, 2010 IEEE Computer Society Conference on Computer Vision and Pattern Recognition.

[38]  F. A. Miles,et al.  Spatial summation properties of the human ocular following response (OFR): Evidence for nonlinearities due to local and global inhibitory interactions , 2008, Vision Research.

[39]  Dario L Ringach,et al.  Dynamics of tuning in the Fourier domain. , 2008, Journal of neurophysiology.

[40]  Stephen G. Lisberger,et al.  Sensory Population Decoding for Visually Guided Movements , 2013, Neuron.

[41]  A Gorea,et al.  Spatial integration characteristics in motion detection and direction identification. , 1985, Spatial vision.

[42]  Stephen G. Lisberger,et al.  The Interaction of Bayesian Priors and Sensory Data and Its Neural Circuit Implementation in Visually Guided Movement , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[43]  W. Newsome,et al.  A selective impairment of motion perception following lesions of the middle temporal visual area (MT) , 1988, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[44]  Mark D'Esposito,et al.  Corrections to: Top–down flow of visual spatial attention signals from parietal to occipital cortex , 2010 .

[45]  S. McKee,et al.  journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/visres , 2022 .

[46]  Frans A. J. Verstraten,et al.  Spatial summation and its interaction with the temporal integration mechanism in human motion perception , 1994, Vision Research.

[47]  D. Robinson,et al.  A METHOD OF MEASURING EYE MOVEMENT USING A SCLERAL SEARCH COIL IN A MAGNETIC FIELD. , 1963, IEEE transactions on bio-medical engineering.

[48]  S. Dakin,et al.  Spatial interference among moving targets , 2005, Vision Research.

[49]  J. Koenderink,et al.  Spatiotemporal integration in the detection of coherent motion , 1984, Vision Research.

[50]  S. Yamane,et al.  Neural activity in cortical area MST of alert monkey during ocular following responses. , 1994, Journal of neurophysiology.

[51]  Karl R Gegenfurtner,et al.  Does the noise matter? Effects of different kinematogram types on smooth pursuit eye movements and perception. , 2010, Journal of vision.

[52]  John H. R. Maunsell,et al.  The connections of the middle temporal visual area (MT) and their relationship to a cortical hierarchy in the macaque monkey , 1983, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[53]  Preeti Verghese,et al.  PII: S0042-6989(98)00033-9 , 1998 .

[54]  E. J. Morris,et al.  Visual motion processing and sensory-motor integration for smooth pursuit eye movements. , 1987, Annual review of neuroscience.

[55]  Andrew T. Smith,et al.  Is global motion really based on spatial integration of local motion signals? , 1994, Vision Research.

[56]  Athanasios Papoulis,et al.  Probability, Random Variables and Stochastic Processes , 1965 .

[57]  M. J. Morgan,et al.  Spatial filtering precedes motion detection , 1992, Nature.

[58]  David C. Burr,et al.  Pooling and segmenting motion signals , 2009, Vision Research.

[59]  W T Newsome,et al.  How Is a Sensory Map Read Out? Effects of Microstimulation in Visual Area MT on Saccades and Smooth Pursuit Eye Movements , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[60]  Robert Sekuler,et al.  Human Vision Favors Centrifugal Motion , 1980, Perception.

[61]  Stephen G Lisberger,et al.  Spatial and temporal integration of visual motion signals for smooth pursuit eye movements in monkeys. , 2009, Journal of neurophysiology.

[62]  I. Ohzawa,et al.  Organization of suppression in receptive fields of neurons in cat visual cortex. , 1992, Journal of neurophysiology.

[63]  J. Maunsell,et al.  Attentional Modulation of Motion Integration of Individual Neurons in the Middle Temporal Visual Area , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[64]  Heinen,et al.  PII: S0042-6989(97)00422-7 , 1998 .

[65]  Stephen J Heinen,et al.  Human smooth pursuit direction discrimination , 1999, Vision Research.

[66]  Peter Neri,et al.  Surround motion silences signals from same-direction motion. , 2009, Journal of neurophysiology.

[67]  William Bialek,et al.  Time Course of Precision in Smooth-Pursuit Eye Movements of Monkeys , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[68]  D. Burr,et al.  Receptive field size of human motion detection units , 1987, Vision Research.

[69]  H. Komatsu,et al.  Modulation of pursuit eye movements by stimulation of cortical areas MT and MST. , 1989, Journal of neurophysiology.

[70]  A. Fuchs,et al.  Relationship between eye acceleration and retinal image velocity during foveal smooth pursuit in man and monkey. , 1981, Journal of neurophysiology.

[71]  S G Lisberger,et al.  Effect of changing feedback delay on spontaneous oscillations in smooth pursuit eye movements of monkeys. , 1992, Journal of neurophysiology.

[72]  R J Krauzlis,et al.  Effects of directional expectations on motion perception and pursuit eye movements , 2001, Visual Neuroscience.

[73]  T. Uka,et al.  Psychophysical evidence for contraction of the range of spatial integration as a mechanism for filtering out spatial noise in a random dot motion display , 2011, Vision Research.

[74]  S G Lisberger,et al.  Shifts in the Population Response in the Middle Temporal Visual Area Parallel Perceptual and Motor Illusions Produced by Apparent Motion , 2001, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[75]  S. Lisberger,et al.  Properties of visual inputs that initiate horizontal smooth pursuit eye movements in monkeys , 1985, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[76]  F. A. Miles,et al.  Human ocular following: evidence that responses to large-field stimuli are limited by local and global inhibitory influences. , 2008, Progress in Brain Research.

[77]  T. Womelsdorf,et al.  Receptive Field Shift and Shrinkage in Macaque Middle Temporal Area through Attentional Gain Modulation , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[78]  K. Nakayama,et al.  Two Distinct Visual Motion Mechanisms for Smooth Pursuit: Evidence from Individual Differences , 2007, Neuron.

[79]  G. W. Kao,et al.  Timing and velocity randomization similarly affect anticipatory pursuit , 2005 .

[80]  R. Born,et al.  Segregation of global and local motion processing in primate middle temporal visual area , 1993, Nature.

[81]  T. Pasternak,et al.  Transient and permanent deficits in motion perception after lesions of cortical areas MT and MST in the macaque monkey. , 1999, Cerebral cortex.

[82]  Eero P. Simoncelli,et al.  Noise characteristics and prior expectations in human visual speed perception , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.

[83]  Stephen G Lisberger,et al.  Visual Guidance of Smooth-Pursuit Eye Movements: Sensation, Action, and What Happens in Between , 2010, Neuron.

[84]  Randolph Blake,et al.  Fine Temporal Properties of Center–Surround Interactions in Motion Revealed by Reverse Correlation , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[85]  F. A. Miles,et al.  Human ocular following initiated by competing image motions: Evidence for a winner-take-all mechanism , 2006, Vision Research.

[86]  Stephen G. Lisberger,et al.  A model of visually-guided smooth pursuit eye movements based on behavioral observations , 1994, Journal of Computational Neuroscience.

[87]  Randolph Blake,et al.  Perceptual consequences of centre–surround antagonism in visual motion processing , 2003, Nature.

[88]  R. Krauzlis,et al.  Shared motion signals for human perceptual decisions and oculomotor actions. , 2003, Journal of vision.

[89]  R. Hetherington The Perception of the Visual World , 1952 .

[90]  Naoshige Uchida,et al.  Odor Representations in Olfactory Cortex: Distributed Rate Coding and Decorrelated Population Activity , 2012, Neuron.

[91]  J A Movshon,et al.  Visual cortical signals supporting smooth pursuit eye movements. , 1990, Cold Spring Harbor Symposia on Quantitative Biology.

[92]  Robert Sekuler,et al.  Coherent global motion percepts from stochastic local motions , 1984, Vision Research.