Basic and Exceptional Calculation Abilities in a Calculating Prodigy: A Case Study

We describe the basic and exceptional calculation abilities of a calculating prodigy whose performances were investigated in single- and multi-digit number multiplication, numerical comparison, raising of powers, and short-term memory tasks. The study shows how the highly efficient long-term memory storage and retrieval processes of this calculating prodigy, his knowledge of calculation algorithms, and his good short-term memory capacity all contribute to his calculation expertise. Moreover, his pattern of performance in tasks based on memory retrieval strategies sheds new light on the problem-size/difficulty effect exhibited by normal subjects in basic arithmetical operations. When direct memory retrieval is the only strategy used by this calculating prodigy to answer a problem, the size of the item and the size of the answer do not affect the response latency. This result supports interpretations of the problem-size/difficulty effect in terms of learning and practice, and challenges interpretations in terms of magnitude itself.

[1]  R. Siegler The perils of averaging data over strategies: An example from children's addition. , 1987 .

[2]  Another calendar savant. , 1988, The British journal of psychiatry : the journal of mental science.

[3]  A. Tsang,et al.  An investigation of the calendar calculation ability of a Chinese calendar savant , 1991, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[4]  N. O’connor,et al.  Idiot savant calendrical calculators: maths or memory? , 1984, Psychological Medicine.

[5]  K. A. Ericsson,et al.  Skill and Working Memory , 1982 .

[6]  A. Jensen,et al.  Speed of information processing in a calculating prodigy , 1990 .

[7]  S. Dehaene Varieties of numerical abilities , 1992, Cognition.

[8]  G. Hitch The role of short-term working memory in mental arithmetic , 1978, Cognitive Psychology.

[9]  Mark H. Ashcraft,et al.  Evidence for retrieval and decision processes in mental addition , 1978 .

[10]  Jeffrey Bisanz,et al.  Selection of Procedures in Mental Addition: Reassessing the Problem Size Effect in Adults , 1996 .

[11]  James V. Hinrichs,et al.  Two-digit number comparison: Use of place information. , 1981 .

[12]  Jeffrey Bisanz,et al.  Multiple routes to solution of single-digit multiplication problems. , 1996 .

[13]  Michael McCloskey,et al.  Models of arithmetic fact retrieval: an evaluation in light of findings from normal and brain-damaged subjects. , 1991, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[14]  Marcus Giaquinto Concepts and Calculation , 1995 .

[15]  Robert J. Crutcher,et al.  The role of deliberate practice in the acquisition of expert performance. , 1993 .

[16]  G. Groen,et al.  A chronometric analysis of simple addition. , 1972 .

[17]  T. Salthouse,et al.  Effects of increased processing demands on age differences in working memory. , 1990 .

[18]  R. Bruyer,et al.  Assessment of visuospatial short-term memory and effect of aging , 1999 .

[19]  Giyoo Hatano,et al.  Digit memory of grand experts in abacus-derived mental calculation , 1983, Cognition.

[20]  Marie-Pascale Noël,et al.  Images of numbers, or “when 98 is upper left and 6 sky blue” , 1992, Cognition.

[21]  N. O’connor,et al.  Idiot savant calendrical calculators: rules and regularities , 1986, Psychological Medicine.

[22]  I. Hunter An exceptional talent for calculative thinking. , 1962, British journal of psychology.

[23]  Steven Bradley Smith,et al.  The great mental calculators : the psychology, methods, and lives of calculating prodigies, past and present , 1985 .

[24]  J. I. Campbell,et al.  Network interference and number-fact retrieval: evidence from children's alphaplication. , 1992, Canadian journal of psychology.

[25]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Is numerical comparison digital? Analogical and symbolic effects in two-digit number comparison. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[26]  Paul Macaruso,et al.  Representing and using numerical information. , 1995, The American psychologist.

[27]  M. Ashcraft Cognitive arithmetic: A review of data and theory , 1992, Cognition.

[28]  D. Geary,et al.  Effects of practice on component processes in complex mental addition. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[29]  Robert Sekuler,et al.  Processing numerical information: A choice time analysis , 1971 .

[30]  K. A. Ericsson,et al.  Expert and exceptional performance: evidence of maximal adaptation to task constraints. , 1996, Annual review of psychology.

[31]  Mark H. Ashcraft,et al.  Textbook Presentations of the Basic Addition Facts , 1986 .

[32]  R. Siegler,et al.  Four aspects of strategic change: contributions to children's learning of multiplication. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[33]  M. McCloskey Cognitive mechanisms in numerical processing: Evidence from acquired dyscalculia , 1992, Cognition.

[34]  P. Macaruso,et al.  Mental calculation in an autistic savant: a case study. , 1997, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[35]  Michael W. Faust Mathematics Anxiety Effects in Simple and Complex Addition , 1996 .

[36]  M. Thioux,et al.  Selective Preservation of Exceptional Arithmetical Knowledge in a Demented Patient , 1999 .

[37]  S Dehaene,et al.  The psychophysics of numerical comparison: A reexamination of apparently incompatible data , 1989, Perception & psychophysics.

[38]  K. A. Ericsson,et al.  Long-term working memory. , 1995, Psychological review.

[39]  Mark H. Ashcraft,et al.  THE FREQUENCY OF ARITHMETIC FACTS IN ELEMENTARY TEXTS: ADDITION AND MULTIPLICATION IN GRADES 1-6 , 1995 .

[40]  Michael McCloskey,et al.  Facts, rules and procedures in normal calculation: Evidence from multiple single-patient studies of impaired arithmetic fact retrieval , 1991, Brain and Cognition.

[41]  Jamie I. D. Campbell,et al.  Chapter 12 Cognitive Number Processing: An Encoding-Complex Perspective , 1992 .

[42]  C. Gallistel,et al.  Preverbal and verbal counting and computation , 1992, Cognition.

[43]  M. Perlmutter,et al.  Cognitive arithmetic: comparison of operations. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[44]  D. Treffert,et al.  The idiot savant: a review of the syndrome. , 1988, The American journal of psychiatry.

[45]  Jamie I. D. Campbell Network interference and mental multiplication. , 1987 .

[46]  G Denes,et al.  Concepts and facts in calculation. , 1994, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[47]  R. Siegler,et al.  Strategy choice procedures and the development of multiplication skill. , 1988, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[48]  H C Longuet-Higgins,et al.  Learning to count: a computational model of language acquisition , 1978, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B. Biological Sciences.

[49]  Gordon D. Logan,et al.  On the relation between production and verification tasks in the psychology of simple arithmetic , 1990 .

[50]  Eric J. Johnson,et al.  The validity of verbal protocols , 1989, Memory & cognition.

[51]  B. Milner,et al.  Interhemispheric differences in the localization of psychological processes in man. , 1971, British medical bulletin.

[52]  Eric A. Jenkins,et al.  How Children Discover New Strategies , 1989 .

[53]  Casey Dorman,et al.  Exceptional calendar calculation ability after early left hemispherectomy , 1991, Brain and Cognition.

[54]  Jamie I. D. Campbell,et al.  Mental multiplication skill: Structure, process, and acquisition. , 1985 .

[55]  T. Benke,et al.  Impaired Arithmetic Facts But Intact Conceptual Knowledge a Single—Case Study of Dyscalculia , 1995, Cortex.

[56]  Calendar calculating in 'idiots savants': how do they do it? , 1988, British journal of psychology.

[57]  N. O’connor,et al.  Factors and primes: a specific numerical ability , 1990, Psychological Medicine.

[58]  Neil Charness,et al.  Acquiring skill at mental calculation in adulthood: a task decomposition , 1988 .