Prefrontal dopamine and the dynamic control of human long-term memory

Dopaminergic projections to the prefrontal cortex support higher-order cognitive functions, and are critically involved in many psychiatric disorders that involve memory deficits, including schizophrenia. The role of prefrontal dopamine in long-term memory, however, is still unclear. We used an imaging genetics approach to examine the hypothesis that dopamine availability in the prefrontal cortex selectively affects the ability to suppress interfering memories. Human participants were scanned via functional magnetic resonance imaging while practicing retrieval of previously studied target information in the face of interference from previously studied non-target information. This retrieval practice (RP) rendered the non-target information less retrievable on a later final test—a phenomenon known as retrieval-induced forgetting (RIF). In total, 54 participants were genotyped for the catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT) Val108/158Met polymorphism. The COMT Val108/158Met genotype showed a selective and linear gene-dose effect on RIF, with the Met allele, which leads to higher prefrontal dopamine availability, being associated with greater RIF. Mirroring the behavioral pattern, the functional magnetic resonance imaging data revealed that Met allele carriers, compared with Val allele carriers, showed a greater response reduction in inhibitory control areas of the right inferior frontal cortex during RP, suggesting that they more efficiently reduced interference. These data support the hypothesis that the cortical dopaminergic system is centrally involved in the dynamic control of human long-term memory, supporting efficient remembering via the adaptive suppression of interfering memories.

[1]  Paul J. Harrison,et al.  Catechol-o-Methyltransferase, Cognition, and Psychosis: Val158Met and Beyond , 2006, Biological Psychiatry.

[2]  Mark W. Greenlee,et al.  Retrieval from Episodic Memory: Neural Mechanisms of Interference Resolution , 2009, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[3]  David Goldman,et al.  The Role of COMT Val158Met in Cognition , 2009, Biological Psychiatry.

[4]  M. Egan,et al.  Effect of Catechol-O-Methyltransferase val158met Genotype on Attentional Control , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[5]  Lars-Göran Nilsson,et al.  COMT Gene Polymorphism Is Associated with Declarative Memory in Adulthood and Old Age , 2004, Behavior genetics.

[6]  Wolfgang M. Pauli,et al.  Computational models of cognitive control , 2010, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[7]  A. Meyer-Lindenberg,et al.  Intermediate phenotypes and genetic mechanisms of psychiatric disorders , 2006, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[8]  David C. Van Essen,et al.  Application of Information Technology: An Integrated Software Suite for Surface-based Analyses of Cerebral Cortex , 2001, J. Am. Medical Informatics Assoc..

[9]  K. Lesch,et al.  Influence of SLC6A3 and COMT variation on neural activation during response inhibition , 2009, Biological Psychology.

[10]  Benjamin C. Storm,et al.  ADHD and retrieval-induced forgetting: Evidence for a deficit in the inhibitory control of memory , 2010, Memory.

[11]  K. Bäuml,et al.  Deutschsprachige Normen für Vertreter von 48 Kategorien. , 1995 .

[12]  Michael C. Anderson,et al.  Inhibitory processes and the control of memory retrieval , 2002, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[13]  J. Lisman,et al.  The Hippocampal-VTA Loop: Controlling the Entry of Information into Long-Term Memory , 2005, Neuron.

[14]  K. Bäuml,et al.  Individual differences in working memory capacity predict retrieval-induced forgetting. , 2011, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[15]  R. Straub,et al.  Effect of COMT Val108/158 Met genotype on frontal lobe function and risk for schizophrenia , 2001, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[16]  Maro G. Machizawa,et al.  Neural measures reveal individual differences in controlling access to working memory , 2005, Nature.

[17]  M. Posner,et al.  Assessing the molecular genetics of attention networks , 2002, BMC Neuroscience.

[18]  Jeffrey D. Karpicke,et al.  The Critical Importance of Retrieval for Learning , 2008, Science.

[19]  J. Savitz,et al.  The molecular genetics of cognition: dopamine, COMT and BDNF , 2006, Genes, brain, and behavior.

[20]  P. Tobler,et al.  Functional imaging of the human dopaminergic midbrain , 2009, Trends in Neurosciences.

[21]  D. Durstewitz,et al.  The Dual-State Theory of Prefrontal Cortex Dopamine Function with Relevance to Catechol-O-Methyltransferase Genotypes and Schizophrenia , 2008, Biological Psychiatry.

[22]  A. Meyer-Lindenberg,et al.  Neural substrates of pleiotropic action of genetic variation in COMT: a meta-analysis , 2010, Molecular Psychiatry.

[23]  Bernhard Pastötter,et al.  Amount of postcue encoding predicts amount of directed forgetting. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[24]  Andreas Heinz,et al.  The Effects of Catechol O-methyltransferase Genotype on Brain Activation Elicited by Affective Stimuli and Cognitive Tasks , 2006, Reviews in the neurosciences.

[25]  Brice A. Kuhl,et al.  Decreased demands on cognitive control reveal the neural processing benefits of forgetting , 2007, Nature Neuroscience.

[26]  M. Owen,et al.  Is COMT a susceptibility gene for schizophrenia? , 2007, Schizophrenia bulletin.

[27]  R. Coppola,et al.  Executive subprocesses in working memory: relationship to catechol-O-methyltransferase Val158Met genotype and schizophrenia. , 2003, Archives of general psychiatry.

[28]  Carlos J Gómez-Ariza,et al.  Retrieval-Induced Forgetting and Executive Control , 2009, Psychological science.

[29]  M Teresa Bajo,et al.  Inhibitory processes in memory are impaired in schizophrenia: evidence from retrieval induced forgetting. , 2009, British journal of psychology.

[30]  Michael C. Anderson,et al.  Remembering can cause forgetting: retrieval dynamics in long-term memory. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[31]  H. Heinze,et al.  The Dopaminergic Midbrain Participates in Human Episodic Memory Formation: Evidence from Genetic Imaging , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[32]  R. McCarley,et al.  Retrieval-induced forgetting in schizophrenia , 2005, Schizophrenia Research.

[33]  Roberto Cabeza,et al.  COMT val108/158 met genotype affects neural but not cognitive processing in healthy individuals. , 2010, Cerebral cortex.

[34]  Terry E. Goldberg,et al.  Genetic Variation in Catechol-O-Methyltransferase: Effects on Working Memory in Schizophrenic Patients, Their Siblings, and Healthy Controls , 2008, Biological Psychiatry.

[35]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Cognition and control in schizophrenia: a computational model of dopamine and prefrontal function , 1999, Biological Psychiatry.

[36]  H. E. Rosvold,et al.  Cognitive deficit caused by regional depletion of dopamine in prefrontal cortex of rhesus monkey. , 1979, Science.

[37]  Zara M. Bergström,et al.  The University of Birmingham ( Live System ) Neural markers of inhibition in human memory retrieval , 2016 .

[38]  A. C. Butler,et al.  The critical role of retrieval practice in long-term retention , 2011, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[39]  E. Düzel,et al.  Chapter 3.8 The multiple roles of dopaminergic neurotransmission in episodic memory , 2008 .

[40]  R. O’Reilly Biologically Based Computational Models of High-Level Cognition , 2006, Science.

[41]  Graham V. Williams,et al.  Inverted-U dopamine D1 receptor actions on prefrontal neurons engaged in working memory , 2007, Nature Neuroscience.

[42]  A. Meyer-Lindenberg,et al.  Prefrontal-Hippocampal Coupling During Memory Processing Is Modulated by COMT Val158Met Genotype , 2006, Biological Psychiatry.

[43]  M. Egan,et al.  Catechol O-methyltransferase val158-met genotype and individual variation in the brain response to amphetamine , 2003, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[44]  Tilo Kircher,et al.  COMT genotype and its role on hippocampal–prefrontal regions in declarative memory , 2010, NeuroImage.

[45]  T. Robbins,et al.  Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[46]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Statistical parametric maps in functional imaging: A general linear approach , 1994 .

[47]  J. Callicott,et al.  Prefrontal cognitive systems in schizophrenia : Towards human genetic brain mechanisms , 2009, Cognitive neuropsychiatry.

[48]  Charan Ranganath,et al.  The Cognitive Neuroscience of Memory Function and Dysfunction in Schizophrenia , 2008, Biological Psychiatry.

[49]  S. Kapur,et al.  The dopamine hypothesis of schizophrenia: version III--the final common pathway. , 2009, Schizophrenia bulletin.

[50]  W. Montague,et al.  Category norms of verbal items in 56 categories A replication and extension of the Connecticut category norms , 1969 .

[51]  T. Klingberg,et al.  Prefrontal cortex and basal ganglia control access to working memory , 2008, Nature Neuroscience.

[52]  Thomas E. Nichols,et al.  Impact of complex genetic variation in COMT on human brain function , 2006, Molecular Psychiatry.

[53]  D. Weinberger,et al.  COMT Val158Met polymorphism, cognitive stability and cognitive flexibility: an experimental examination , 2010, Behavioral and Brain Functions.

[54]  A. Diamond,et al.  Genetic and neurochemical modulation of prefrontal cognitive functions in children. , 2004, The American journal of psychiatry.

[55]  K. Bäuml,et al.  Theta Oscillations Reflect the Dynamics of Interference in Episodic Memory Retrieval , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.