Catching optical information for the regulation of timing
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] W. Schiff,et al. Aging persons' estimates of vehicular motion. , 1992, Psychology and aging.
[2] M. Cynader,et al. The visual perception of motion in depth. , 1979, Scientific American.
[3] P R DeLucia,et al. Pictorial and motion-based information for depth perception. , 1991, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[4] J. A. Scott Kelso,et al. The perceptual guidance of grasping a moving object , 1992 .
[5] Andrea H. Mason,et al. Target viewing time and velocity effects on prehension , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.
[6] E Brenner,et al. Hitting moving objects. The dependency of hand velocity on the speed of the target. , 2000, Experimental brain research.
[7] D. Robertson,et al. The kinematics, movement phasing and timing of a skilled action in response to varying conditions of uncertainty , 1985 .
[8] D. Regan,et al. Binocular and monocular stimuli for motion in depth: Changing-disparity and changing-size feed the same motion-in-depth stage , 1979, Vision Research.
[9] Joan López-Moliner,et al. Speed of response initiation in a time-to-contact discrimination task reflects the use of η , 2002, Vision Research.
[10] V Cavallo,et al. Visual Information and Skill Level in Time-To-Collision Estimation , 1988, Perception.
[11] Jeroen B. J. Smeets,et al. Multiple information sources in interceptive timing , 1997 .
[12] M. Laurent,et al. Perception-action coupling in an interceptive task: First-order time-to-contact as an input variable , 2000 .
[13] K. Rosengren,et al. The Separation of Action and Perception and the Issue of Affordances , 2001 .
[14] P R DeLucia,et al. Pictorial and motion-based depth information during active control of self-motion: size-arrival effects on collision avoidance. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[15] C. Michaels. Information, Perception, and Action: What Should Ecological Psychologists Learn From Milner and Goodale (1995)? , 2000 .
[16] J. Tresilian,et al. Temporal precision of interceptive action: differential effects of target size and speed , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.
[17] F. C. Bakker,et al. Catching balls: how to get the hand to the right place at the right time. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[18] Daniel Bullock,et al. Prospective control of manual interceptive actions: comparative simulations of extant and new model constructs , 2001, Neural Networks.
[19] Scott M. Dittman,et al. Monocular optical constraints on collision control. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[20] H. Ross,et al. Optic-Flow and Cognitive Factors in Time-to-Collision Estimates , 1983, Perception.
[21] B. Frost,et al. Computation of different optical variables of looming objects in pigeon nucleus rotundus neurons , 1998, Nature Neuroscience.
[22] Ben Sidaway,et al. Time-to-Collision Estimation in a Simulated Driving Task , 1996, Hum. Factors.
[23] James R. Tresilian,et al. Approximate information sources and perceptual variables in interceptive timing. , 1994 .
[24] J. Flanagan,et al. Grip-load force coupling: a general control strategy for transporting objects. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[25] Alfred W. Hubbard,et al. Visual Movements of Batters , 1954 .
[26] R. Bootsma,et al. Timing an attacking forehand drive in table tennis. , 1990 .
[27] David N. Lee,et al. Visual Timing in Hitting An Accelerating Ball , 1983, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.
[28] J. Tresilian,et al. a moving target: effects of temporal precision constraints and movement amplitude , 2022 .
[29] John P. Wann,et al. Anticipating arrival: is the tau margin a specious theory? , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[30] J. Tresilian. Visually timed action: time-out for ‘tau’? , 1999, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[31] S Bennett,et al. Timing a one-handed catch. I. Effects of telestereoscopic viewing. , 1999, Experimental brain research.
[32] Timothy D. Lee,et al. Motor Control and Learning: A Behavioral Emphasis , 1982 .
[33] A. Remole. PERCEPTION WITH AN EYE FOR MOTION , 1987 .
[34] Simone R. Caljouw,et al. Timing of goal-directed hitting: impact requirements change the information–movement coupling , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.
[35] R J Bootsma,et al. Visual information about time-to-collision between two objects. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[36] G. Savelsbergh,et al. Timing a one-handed catch. II. Adaptation to telestereoscopic viewing. , 1999, Experimental brain research.
[37] H. Wagner. Flow-field variables trigger landing in flies , 1982, Nature.
[38] B J McFadyen,et al. Visuomotor control when reaching toward and grasping moving targets. , 1996, Acta psychologica.
[39] G. J. Savelsbergh,et al. Grasping tau. , 1991, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[40] W. Schiff. PERCEPTION OF IMPENDING COLLISION: A STUDY OF VISUALLY DIRECTED AVOIDANT BEHAVIOR. , 1965, Psychological monographs.
[41] H Collewijn,et al. Binocular eye movements and the perception of depth. , 1990, Reviews of oculomotor research.
[42] M. Goodale,et al. The visual brain in action , 1995 .
[43] R. Bootsma,et al. On the information-based regulation of movement: What Wann (1996) may want to consider , 1997 .
[44] James A. Caviness,et al. Persistent Fear Responses in Rhesus Monkeys to the Optical Stimulus of "Looming" , 1962, Science.
[45] Alain Durey,et al. Binocular Invariants in Interceptive Tasks: A Directed Perception Approach , 1996 .
[46] Reinoud J Bootsma,et al. Global and Local Contributions to the Optical Specification of Time to Contact: Observer Sensitivity to Composite Tau , 2002, Perception.
[47] W. Warren. Action modes and laws of control for the visual guidance of action , 1988 .
[48] D Regan,et al. Accuracy of estimating time to collision using binocular and monocular information , 1998, Vision Research.
[49] Keith Davids,et al. Discriminating the role of binocular information in the timing of a one-handed catch , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.
[50] L. Fogassi,et al. Prehension movements directed to approaching objects: Influence of stimulus velocity on the transport and the grasp components , 1992, Neuropsychologia.
[51] A. M. Burden,et al. The role of predictive visual temporal information in the coordination of muscle activity in catching , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.
[52] David N. Lee,et al. A Theory of Visual Control of Braking Based on Information about Time-to-Collision , 1976, Perception.
[53] C. Michaels,et al. Information and action in punching a falling ball , 2001, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.
[54] E Brenner,et al. Perception and action are based on the same visual information: distinction between position and velocity. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[55] A Dubrowski,et al. Control Strategies When Intercepting Slowly Moving Targets , 2001, Journal of motor behavior.
[56] Bruce Abernethy,et al. Timing structure in squash strokes: Further evidence for the operational timing hypothesis , 1988 .
[57] G. Montagne,et al. The control and coordination of one-handed catching: the effect of temporal constraints , 1994, Experimental Brain Research.
[58] David N. Lee,et al. Plummeting gannets: a paradigm of ecological optics , 1981, Nature.
[59] H. Heuer,et al. Estimates of Time to Contact Based on Changing Size and Changing Target Vergence , 1993, Perception.
[60] A. Wing,et al. Grasp size and accuracy of approach in reaching. , 1986, Journal of motor behavior.