Retrieving Information from Human Movement Patterns

Biological motion—that is, the movement patterns of animals and humans—provides a rich source of information that helps us to quickly and reliably detect the presence of another living being; to identify it as a predator, prey, or conspecific; and to infer its actions and intentions in order to respond with adequate behavior. Once we know that we are being confronted with another person, we are able to use motion as a source of information about identity, gender, age, emotional state, and personality traits and as a complex means for signaling and communications. More than 30 years ago, the Swedish psychologist Gunnar Johans-son (1973) introduced a stimulus into experimental psychology that allows us to disentangle to a large degree the information contained in the kinematics of a moving body from other sources of information about action and identity. His work showed that a few light dots placed strategically on a moving human or animal body are instantaneously organized into the coherent percept of a living creature (also see Chapter 11 in this volume). The observation goes back to the earlier work of the pioneers of cinematography (Muybridge, 1887/1979) and bio-mechanics (Marey, 1895/1972), but it was Johansson (1973) who first

[1]  Randolph Blake,et al.  Eccentric perception of biological motion is unscalably poor , 2005, Vision Research.

[2]  Ronald A. Rensink,et al.  Active versus passive processing of biological motion , 2002, Perception.

[3]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Neural Circuits Involved in the Recognition of Actions Performed by Nonconspecifics: An fMRI Study , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[4]  T. Allison,et al.  Temporal Cortex Activation in Humans Viewing Eye and Mouth Movements , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[5]  M. Farah,et al.  What causes the face inversion effect? , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[6]  Ken-ichi Anjyo,et al.  Fourier principles for emotion-based human figure animation , 1995, SIGGRAPH.

[7]  M. Giese,et al.  Learning to discriminate complex movements: biological versus artificial trajectories. , 2006, Journal of vision.

[8]  Charles F. Rose,et al.  Verbs and adverbs: multidimensional motion interpolation using radial basis functions , 1999 .

[9]  Natasha Loder,et al.  Journal under attack over controversial paper on GM food , 1999, Nature.

[10]  N. Troje Decomposing biological motion: a framework for analysis and synthesis of human gait patterns. , 2002, Journal of vision.

[11]  G. Johansson Visual perception of biological motion and a model for its analysis , 1973 .

[12]  S. Lea,et al.  Categorization of natural movements by pigeons: visual concept discrimination and biological motion. , 1998, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[13]  Lance Williams,et al.  Motion signal processing , 1995, SIGGRAPH.

[14]  J E Cutting,et al.  A biomechanical invariant for gait perception. , 1978, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[15]  P. Sinha,et al.  Functional neuroanatomy of biological motion perception in humans , 2001, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[16]  Nikolaus F. Troje,et al.  Separation of texture and shape in images of faces for image coding and synthesis , 1997 .

[17]  J. Decety,et al.  Top down effect of strategy on the perception of human biological motion: a pet investigation. , 1998, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[18]  Tomaso A. Poggio,et al.  Linear Object Classes and Image Synthesis From a Single Example Image , 1997, IEEE Trans. Pattern Anal. Mach. Intell..

[19]  N. Troje,et al.  Person identification from biological motion: Effects of structural and kinematic cues , 2005, Perception & psychophysics.

[20]  R. Fox,et al.  The perception of biological motion by human infants. , 1982, Science.

[21]  G Johansson,et al.  Spatio-temporal differentiation and integration in visual motion perception , 1976, Psychological research.

[22]  Michael S Beauchamp,et al.  See me, hear me, touch me: multisensory integration in lateral occipital-temporal cortex , 2005, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[23]  N. Kanwisher,et al.  The Human Body , 2001 .

[24]  R. M. Alexander,et al.  Optimization and gaits in the locomotion of vertebrates. , 1989, Physiological reviews.

[25]  Christian R. Shelton,et al.  Morphable Surface Models , 2000, International Journal of Computer Vision.

[26]  M. Lemke,et al.  Spatiotemporal gait patterns during over ground locomotion in major depression compared with healthy controls. , 2000, Journal of psychiatric research.

[27]  T. Vetter,et al.  Representations of human faces , 1996 .

[28]  D R Proffitt,et al.  Perception of biomechanical motions by infants: implementation of various processing constraints. , 1987, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[29]  R. Blake,et al.  Brain Areas Active during Visual Perception of Biological Motion , 2002, Neuron.

[30]  S T Roweis,et al.  Nonlinear dimensionality reduction by locally linear embedding. , 2000, Science.

[31]  D. Perrett,et al.  Responses of Anterior Superior Temporal Polysensory (STPa) Neurons to Biological Motion Stimuli , 1994, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[32]  Randolph Blake,et al.  Learning to See Biological Motion: Brain Activity Parallels Behavior , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[33]  J E Cutting,et al.  Masking the motions of human gait , 1988, Perception & psychophysics.

[34]  B. Silverman,et al.  Functional Data Analysis , 1997 .

[35]  Giorgio Vallortigara,et al.  Visually Inexperienced Chicks Exhibit Spontaneous Preference for Biological Motion Patterns , 2005, PLoS biology.

[36]  R. B. Davis,et al.  A gait analysis data collection and reduction technique , 1991 .

[37]  Armin Bruderlin,et al.  Perceiving affect from arm movement , 2001, Cognition.

[38]  Nello Cristianini,et al.  An Introduction to Support Vector Machines and Other Kernel-based Learning Methods , 2000 .

[39]  Giorgio Vallortigara,et al.  Gravity bias in the interpretation of biological motion by inexperienced chicks , 2006, Current Biology.

[40]  J. Cutting,et al.  Recognizing the sex of a walker from a dynamic point-light display , 1977 .

[41]  Pascal Fua,et al.  Style‐Based Motion Synthesis † , 2004, Comput. Graph. Forum.

[42]  M. Golubitsky,et al.  Symmetry in locomotor central pattern generators and animal gaits , 1999, Nature.

[43]  Ian Stewart,et al.  A modular network for legged locomotion , 1998 .

[44]  Pierre Fonlupt,et al.  Listening to a walking human activates the temporal biological motion area , 2005, NeuroImage.

[45]  Michael Jones,et al.  Multidimensional Morphable Models: A Framework for Representing and Matching Object Classes , 2004, International Journal of Computer Vision.

[46]  R. Blake Cats Perceive Biological Motion , 1993 .

[47]  Zoran Popovic,et al.  Motion warping , 1995, SIGGRAPH.

[48]  J. Cutting,et al.  Recognizing friends by their walk: Gait perception without familiarity cues , 1977 .

[49]  S. Lea,et al.  Perception of Emotion from Dynamic Point-Light Displays Represented in Dance , 1996, Perception.

[50]  G. Orban,et al.  Specificity of regions processing biological motion , 2005, The European journal of neuroscience.

[51]  Michael J. Black,et al.  Robust Principal Component Analysis for Computer Vision , 2001, ICCV.

[52]  N. Troje,et al.  The Inversion Effect in Biological Motion Perception: Evidence for a “Life Detector”? , 2006, Current Biology.

[53]  Martin A. Giese,et al.  Morphable Models for the Analysis and Synthesis of Complex Motion Patterns , 2000, International Journal of Computer Vision.

[54]  J. Cutting Generation of Synthetic Male and Female Walkers through Manipulation of a Biomechanical Invariant , 1978, Perception.

[55]  Shang Guo,et al.  A high-level control mechanism for human locomotion based on parametric frame space interpolation , 1996 .

[56]  Eleanor Rosch,et al.  Principles of Categorization , 1978 .

[57]  J. Haxby,et al.  Parallel Visual Motion Processing Streams for Manipulable Objects and Human Movements , 2002, Neuron.

[58]  P. Downing,et al.  Selectivity for the human body in the fusiform gyrus. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[59]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  Specific Involvement of Human Parietal Systems and the Amygdala in the Perception of Biological Motion , 1996, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[60]  R. Blake,et al.  Brain Areas Involved in Perception of Biological Motion , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[61]  James W. Tanaka,et al.  What causes the face inversion effect , 1995 .

[62]  G. Mather,et al.  Gender discrimination in biological motion displays based on dynamic cues , 1994, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[63]  P. Cavanagh,et al.  Attention-based visual routines: sprites , 2001, Cognition.

[64]  Eadweard Muybridge,et al.  Muybridge's Complete human and animal locomotion : all 781 plates from the 1887 Animal locomotion , 1979 .

[65]  J. Cutting,et al.  Temporal and spatial factors in gait perception that influence gender recognition , 1978, Perception & psychophysics.