Directing Voluntary Temporal Attention Increases Fixational Stability

Our visual input is constantly changing, but not all moments are equally relevant. Visual temporal attention, the prioritization of visual information at specific points in time, increases perceptual sensitivity at behaviorally relevant times. The dynamic processes underlying this increase are unclear. During fixation, humans make small eye movements called microsaccades, and inhibiting microsaccades improves perception of brief stimuli. Here, we investigated whether temporal attention changes the pattern of microsaccades in anticipation of brief stimuli. Human observers (female and male) judged stimuli presented within a short sequence. Observers were given either an informative precue to attend to one of the stimuli, which was likely to be probed, or an uninformative (neutral) precue. We found strong microsaccadic inhibition before the stimulus sequence, likely due to its predictable onset. Critically, this anticipatory inhibition was stronger when the first target in the sequence (T1) was precued (task-relevant) than when the precue was uninformative. Moreover, the timing of the last microsaccade before T1 and the first microsaccade after T1 shifted such that both occurred earlier when T1 was precued than when the precue was uninformative. Finally, the timing of the nearest pre- and post-T1 microsaccades affected task performance. Directing voluntary temporal attention therefore affects microsaccades, helping to stabilize fixation at the most relevant moments over and above the effect of predictability. Just as saccading to a relevant stimulus can be an overt correlate of the allocation of spatial attention, precisely timed gaze stabilization can be an overt correlate of the allocation of temporal attention. SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT We pay attention at moments in time when a relevant event is likely to occur. Such temporal attention improves our visual perception, but how it does so is not well understood. Here, we discovered a new behavioral correlate of voluntary, or goal-directed, temporal attention. We found that the pattern of small fixational eye movements called microsaccades changes around behaviorally relevant moments in a way that stabilizes the position of the eyes. Microsaccades during a brief visual stimulus can impair perception of that stimulus. Therefore, such fixation stabilization may contribute to the improvement of visual perception at attended times. This link suggests that, in addition to cortical areas, subcortical areas mediating eye movements may be recruited with temporal attention.

[1]  J NACHMIAS,et al.  Two-dimensional motion of the retinal image during monocular fixation. , 1959, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[2]  A. Nobre,et al.  Orienting attention in time. Modulation of brain potentials. , 1999, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[3]  B. Postle,et al.  Top-down control of the phase of alpha-band oscillations as a mechanism for temporal prediction , 2015, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[4]  Nikos K. Logothetis,et al.  The Distribution of Microsaccade Directions Need Not Reveal the Location of Attention: Reply to Rolfs, Engbert, and Kliegl , 2004 .

[5]  Robert Desimone,et al.  Enhanced Neural Processing by Covert Attention only during Microsaccades Directed toward the Attended Stimulus , 2018, Neuron.

[6]  Francisco M. Costela,et al.  Task difficulty in mental arithmetic affects microsaccadic rates and magnitudes , 2014, The European journal of neuroscience.

[7]  Rolf Ulrich,et al.  Locus of the effect of temporal preparation: evidence from the lateralized readiness potential. , 2003, Psychophysiology.

[8]  Robin Walker,et al.  What determines the direction of microsaccades , 2010 .

[9]  M. Turatto,et al.  Are you ready? I can tell by looking at your microsaccades , 2006, Neuroreport.

[10]  Marisa Carrasco,et al.  Oculomotor inhibition reflects temporal expectations , 2019, NeuroImage.

[11]  Dekel Abeles,et al.  Temporal dynamics of saccades explained by a self-paced process , 2017, Scientific Reports.

[12]  D H Brainard,et al.  The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[13]  Marisa Carrasco,et al.  Selective attention within the foveola , 2017, Nature Neuroscience.

[14]  Tobias Egner,et al.  Feature-Based Attention and Feature-Based Expectation , 2016, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[15]  Christian N L Olivers,et al.  Oculomotor measures reveal the temporal dynamics of preparing for search. , 2017, Progress in brain research.

[16]  Barbara J. Winterson,et al.  Microsaccades during finely guided visuomotor tasks , 1976, Vision Research.

[17]  R. W. DITCHBURN,et al.  Vision with a Stabilized Retinal Image , 1952, Nature.

[18]  J. Tenenbaum,et al.  Spatial Attention and Temporal Expectation Under Timed Uncertainty Predictably Modulate Neuronal Responses in Monkey V1. , 2015, Cerebral cortex.

[19]  Janneke F. M. Jehee,et al.  Attention Reverses the Effect of Prediction in Silencing Sensory Signals , 2011, Cerebral cortex.

[20]  Anna C. Nobre,et al.  Anticipated moments: temporal structure in attention , 2017, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[21]  M. Rolfs Microsaccades: Small steps on a long way , 2009, Vision Research.

[22]  Britt Anderson,et al.  Effects of temporal context and temporal expectancy on neural activity in inferior temporal cortex , 2008, Neuropsychologia.

[23]  Uri Polat,et al.  Contrast sensitivity revealed by microsaccades. , 2015, Journal of vision.

[24]  Christopher Summerfield,et al.  Dissociable prior influences of signal probability and relevance on visual contrast sensitivity , 2011 .

[25]  Marisa Carrasco,et al.  Prestimulus Inhibition of Saccades in Adults With and Without Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder as an Index of Temporal Expectations , 2017, Psychological science.

[26]  Xoana G. Troncoso,et al.  Microsaccades Counteract Visual Fading during Fixation , 2005, Neuron.

[27]  Xoana G. Troncoso,et al.  Saccades and microsaccades during visual fixation, exploration, and search: foundations for a common saccadic generator. , 2008, Journal of vision.

[28]  D G Pelli,et al.  The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[29]  S. Martinez-Conde,et al.  The impact of microsaccades on vision: towards a unified theory of saccadic function , 2013, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[30]  W. Singer,et al.  Gamma Responses Correlate with Temporal Expectation in Monkey Primary Visual Cortex , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[31]  A. Nobre,et al.  Time for the Fourth Dimension in Attention , 2014 .

[32]  Daniel P. Newman,et al.  Antagonistic Interactions Between Microsaccades and Evidence Accumulation Processes During Decision Formation , 2018, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[33]  G. W. Beeler,et al.  Visual threshold changes resulting from spontaneous saccadic eye movements. , 1967, Vision research.

[34]  J. Lupiáñez,et al.  Attentional preparation based on temporal expectancy modulates processing at the perceptual level , 2005, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[35]  L. Riggs,et al.  The disappearance of steadily fixated visual test objects. , 1953, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[36]  James J. Clark,et al.  Microsaccades as an overt measure of covert attention shifts , 2002, Vision Research.

[37]  Wolfgang Einhäuser,et al.  Reward modulates perception in binocular rivalry. , 2015, Journal of vision.

[38]  Franck Vidal,et al.  Orienting Attention in Time Activates Left Intraparietal Sulcus for Both Perceptual and Motor Task Goals , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[39]  Marisa Carrasco,et al.  Attention flexibly trades off across points in time , 2017, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[40]  Ziad M. Hafed,et al.  Vision, Perception, and Attention through the Lens of Microsaccades: Mechanisms and Implications , 2015, Front. Syst. Neurosci..

[41]  R. Steinman,et al.  Voluntary Control of Microsaccades during Maintained Monocular Fixation , 1967, Science.

[42]  Anna C Nobre,et al.  Combining spatial and temporal expectations to improve visual perception. , 2014, Journal of vision.

[43]  Ziad M. Hafed,et al.  A neural locus for spatial-frequency specific saccadic suppression in visual-motor neurons of the primate superior colliculus. , 2017, Journal of neurophysiology.

[44]  Francisco M. Costela,et al.  Changes in visibility as a function of spatial frequency and microsaccade occurrence , 2017, The European journal of neuroscience.

[45]  J. Braun,et al.  Rare but precious: Microsaccades are highly informative about attentional allocation , 2010, Vision Research.

[46]  M. Belmonte Potentials , 2021, Quantum Mechanics.

[47]  C. Summerfield,et al.  Expectation (and attention) in visual cognition , 2009, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[48]  A. Nobre,et al.  Endogenous modulation of low frequency oscillations by temporal expectations , 2011, Journal of neurophysiology.

[49]  Ziad M. Hafed,et al.  Neuronal Response Gain Enhancement prior to Microsaccades , 2015, Current Biology.

[50]  George Wood Beeler,et al.  Stochastic processes in the human eye movement control system , 1965 .

[51]  D. Heeger,et al.  Spontaneous Microsaccades Reflect Shifts in Covert Attention , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[52]  Lance M. Optican,et al.  Modeling the Triggering of Saccades, Microsaccades, and Saccadic Intrusions , 2018, Front. Neurol..

[53]  U. Polat,et al.  ADHD subjects fail to suppress eye blinks and microsaccades while anticipating visual stimuli but recover with medication , 2014, Vision Research.

[54]  J. Lupiáñez,et al.  Temporal attention enhances early visual processing: A review and new evidence from event-related potentials , 2006, Brain Research.

[55]  Martina Poletti,et al.  Microscopic Eye Movements Compensate for Nonhomogeneous Vision within the Fovea , 2013, Current Biology.

[56]  Ziad M. Hafed,et al.  A Neural Mechanism for Microsaccade Generation in the Primate Superior Colliculus , 2009, Science.

[57]  Mark G. Stokes,et al.  Functionally dissociating temporal and motor components of response preparation in left intraparietal sulcus , 2011, NeuroImage.

[58]  Anna C Nobre,et al.  Decoding the influence of anticipatory states on visual perception in the presence of temporal distractors , 2018, Nature Communications.

[59]  Ziad M. Hafed,et al.  Modulation of Microsaccades in Monkey during a Covert Visual Attention Task , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[60]  Ralf Engbert,et al.  Microsaccades uncover the orientation of covert attention , 2003, Vision Research.

[61]  Shlomit Yuval-Greenberg,et al.  Scalp-Recorded Induced Gamma-Band Responses to Auditory Stimulation and Its Correlations with Saccadic Muscle-Activity , 2011, Brain Topography.

[62]  Ralf Engbert,et al.  Toward a model of microsaccade generation: the case of microsaccadic inhibition. , 2008, Journal of vision.

[63]  R. Miall,et al.  The precision of temporal judgement: milliseconds, many minutes, and beyond , 2009, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[64]  R. Steinman,et al.  The smallest voluntary saccade: implications for fixation. , 1973, Vision research.

[65]  J M Findlay,et al.  Direction perception and human fixation eye movements. , 1974, Vision research.

[66]  B. L. Zuber,et al.  Saccadic suppression: elevation of visual threshold associated with saccadic eye movements. , 1966, Experimental neurology.

[67]  A. Nobre,et al.  Multiple mechanisms of selective attention: differential modulation of stimulus processing by attention to space or time , 2002, Neuropsychologia.

[68]  Francisco M. Costela,et al.  V1 neurons respond differently to object motion versus motion from eye movements , 2015, Nature Communications.

[69]  Rachel N. Denison,et al.  Temporal attention improves perception similarly at foveal and parafoveal locations , 2019, Journal of vision.

[70]  A. Nobre,et al.  Where and When to Pay Attention: The Neural Systems for Directing Attention to Spatial Locations and to Time Intervals as Revealed by Both PET and fMRI , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[71]  S. Grondin Timing and time perception: A review of recent behavioral and neuroscience findings and theoretical directions , 2010, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[72]  Martina Poletti,et al.  Control and Functions of Fixational Eye Movements. , 2015, Annual review of vision science.

[73]  Geoffrey M. Ghose,et al.  Attentional modulation in visual cortex depends on task timing , 2002, Nature.

[74]  Xoana G. Troncoso,et al.  Microsaccadic Efficacy and Contribution to Foveal and Peripheral Vision , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[75]  Todd M. Herrington,et al.  The Effect of Microsaccades on the Correlation between Neural Activity and Behavior in Middle Temporal, Ventral Intraparietal, and Lateral Intraparietal Areas , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[76]  R. Oostenveld,et al.  Nonparametric statistical testing of EEG- and MEG-data , 2007, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[77]  Denis G. Pelli,et al.  ECVP '07 Abstracts , 2007, Perception.

[78]  Ziad M. Hafed,et al.  Microsaccadic Suppression of Visual Bursts in the Primate Superior Colliculus , 2010, Journal of Neuroscience.

[79]  Vincenza Tarantino,et al.  Spatiotemporal Neurodynamics Underlying Internally and Externally Driven Temporal Prediction: A High Spatial Resolution ERP Study , 2015, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[80]  Michael J. Morais,et al.  Dynamics of excitatory and inhibitory networks are differentially altered by selective attention. , 2016, Journal of neurophysiology.