Event-related potential responses to perceptual reversals are modulated by working memory load
暂无分享,去创建一个
Miguel Castelo-Branco | Mika Koivisto | M. Koivisto | M. Castelo‐Branco | Monika Intaitė | M. Intaitė
[1] Michael Bach,et al. Necker cube: stimulus-related (low-level) and percept-related (high-level) EEG signatures early in occipital cortex. , 2011, Journal of vision.
[2] J. Qiu,et al. The vase-face illusion seen by the brain: an event-related brain potentials study. , 2009, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.
[3] Peter König,et al. Influence of disparity on fixation and saccades in free viewing of natural scenes. , 2009, Journal of vision.
[4] G. J. Brouwer,et al. Voluntary control and the dynamics of perceptual bi-stability , 2005, Vision Research.
[5] J. Braun,et al. Structure-from-motion: dissociating perception, neural persistence, and sensory memory of illusory depth and illusory rotation , 2012, Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics.
[6] Mary A. Peterson,et al. Opposed-set measurement procedure: A quantitative analysis of the role of local cues and intention in form perception. , 1983 .
[7] J. Hochberg,et al. Piecemeal organization and cognitive components in object perception: perceptually coupled responses to moving objects. , 1987, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[8] A. Kok. On the utility of P3 amplitude as a measure of processing capacity. , 2001, Psychophysiology.
[9] R. Liebert,et al. Voluntary Control of Reversible Figures , 1985, Perceptual and motor skills.
[10] Edward K. Vogel,et al. The capacity of visual working memory for features and conjunctions , 1997, Nature.
[11] L. Jonkman,et al. Effects of concurrent working memory load on distractor and conflict processing in a name-face Stroop task. , 2011, Psychophysiology.
[12] Osvaldas Rukšėnas,et al. Reversal negativity and bistable stimuli: Attention, awareness, or something else? , 2010, Brain and Cognition.
[13] Miguel Castelo-Branco,et al. The linear impact of concurrent working memory load on dynamics of Necker cube perceptual reversals. , 2014, Journal of vision.
[14] R. van Ee,et al. Percept-choice sequences driven by interrupted ambiguous stimuli: a low-level neural model. , 2007, Journal of vision.
[15] Georgios A. Keliris,et al. Introduction to Research Topic – Binocular Rivalry: A Gateway to Studying Consciousness , 2012, Front. Hum. Neurosci..
[16] Michael Bach,et al. Discontinuous presentation of ambiguous figures: how interstimulus-interval durations affect reversal dynamics and ERPs. , 2007, Psychophysiology.
[17] G. E. Smith. The Human Brain , 1924, Nature.
[18] J. Jonides,et al. Rehearsal in spatial working memory. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[19] M. Ernst. Learning to integrate arbitrary signals from vision and touch. , 2007, Journal of vision.
[20] E. Donchin,et al. Is the P300 component a manifestation of context updating? , 1988, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[21] Maro G. Machizawa,et al. Neural activity predicts individual differences in visual working memory capacity , 2004, Nature.
[22] Geraint Rees,et al. A Neural Basis for Percept Stabilization in Binocular Rivalry , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[23] E. Vogel,et al. CURRENT DIRECTIONS IN PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE How to Exploit Diversity for Scientific Gain Using Individual Differences to Constrain Cognitive Theory , 2022 .
[24] Joel Pearson,et al. Sensory memory for ambiguous vision , 2008, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[25] Sabine Windmann,et al. Role of the Prefrontal Cortex in Attentional Control over Bistable Vision , 2006 .
[26] Michael Bach,et al. Multistable perception: When bottom-up and top-down coincide , 2009, Brain and Cognition.
[27] V. Noreika,et al. Interaction of bottom-up and top-down processes in the perception of ambiguous figures , 2013, Vision Research.
[28] T. Knapen,et al. The spatial scale of perceptual memory in ambiguous figure perception. , 2009, Journal of vision.
[29] M M Smyth,et al. Interference in immediate spatial memory , 1994, Memory & cognition.
[30] David Soto,et al. Electrophysiological evidence for attentional guidance by the contents of working memory , 2009, The European journal of neuroscience.
[31] Maro G. Machizawa,et al. Neural measures reveal individual differences in controlling access to working memory , 2005, Nature.
[32] Barry Fowler,et al. The differential effects of Sternberg short- and long-term memory scanning on the late Nd and P300 in a dual-task paradigm. , 2004, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.
[33] N. Logothetis,et al. Multistable phenomena: changing views in perception , 1999, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[34] R. S. J. Frackowiak,et al. Human brain activity during spontaneously reversing perception of ambiguous figures , 1998, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.
[35] D. Strüber,et al. Differences in Top—Down Influences on the Reversal Rate of Different Categories of Reversible Figures , 1999, Perception.
[36] N. Cowan. The magical number 4 in short-term memory: A reconsideration of mental storage capacity , 2001, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[37] E Başar,et al. Multistable visual perception induces a slow positive EEG wave. , 1993, The International journal of neuroscience.
[38] M. Koivisto,et al. Perceptual reversals of Necker stimuli during intermittent presentation with limited attentional resources. , 2013, Psychophysiology.
[39] Theodor Landis,et al. Right parietal brain activity precedes perceptual alternation of bistable stimuli. , 2009, Cerebral cortex.
[40] J. Jonides,et al. Overlapping mechanisms of attention and spatial working memory , 2001, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[41] A. V. van den Berg,et al. Multi-Timescale Perceptual History Resolves Visual Ambiguity , 2008, PloS one.
[42] Michael A. Pitts,et al. Early top-down influences on bistable perception revealed by event-related potentials , 2008, Brain and Cognition.
[43] C. Koch,et al. Inferring the direction of implied motion depends on visual awareness. , 2014, Journal of vision.
[44] G. Rees,et al. Covariation of activity in visual and prefrontal cortex associated with subjective visual perception. , 1999, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[45] E Donchin,et al. Resource reciprocity: an event-related brain potentials analysis. , 1989, Acta psychologica.
[46] David A. Leopold,et al. Stable perception of visually ambiguous patterns , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.
[47] Thomas C. Toppino,et al. As the cube turns: Evidence for two processes in the perception of a dynamic reversible figure , 1983, Perception & psychophysics.
[48] R. C. Oldfield. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.
[49] J. Orbach,et al. Reversibility of the Necker Cube: III. Effects of Interpolation on Reversal Rate of the Cube Presented Repetitively , 1963, Perceptual and motor skills.
[50] Philip Meier,et al. Rat performance on visual detection task modeled with divisive normalization and adaptive decision thresholds. , 2011, Journal of vision.
[51] Michael Bach,et al. Early neural activity in Necker-cube reversal: evidence for low-level processing of a gestalt phenomenon. , 2004, Psychophysiology.
[52] Michael Bach,et al. The Necker cube—an ambiguous figure disambiguated in early visual processing , 2005, Vision Research.
[53] G. Rees,et al. The Neural Bases of Multistable Perception , 2022 .
[54] Ronald Olson,et al. Reversibility of the Necker Cube: VII. Reversal Rate as a Function of Figure-on and Figure-off Durations , 1966 .
[55] J. Orbach,et al. Reversibility of the Necker Cube: I. An Examination of the Concept of “Satiation of Orientation” , 1963, Perceptual and motor skills.
[56] Thomas C. Toppino,et al. Selective adaptation with reversible figures: Don’t change that channel , 1987, Perception & psychophysics.
[57] J. Buitelaar,et al. Attentional capacity, a probe ERP study: differences between children with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder and normal control children and effects of methylphenidate. , 2000, Psychophysiology.
[58] Michael A. Pitts,et al. Electrophysiological correlates of perceptual reversals for three different types of multistable images. , 2007, Journal of vision.
[59] Alexander Maier,et al. Perception of Temporally Interleaved Ambiguous Patterns , 2003, Current Biology.
[60] Michael Bach,et al. Ambiguous Figures – What Happens in the Brain When Perception Changes But Not the Stimulus , 2011, Front. Hum. Neurosci..
[61] L. H. Pelton,et al. Acceleration of reversals of a Necker cube. , 1968, The American journal of psychology.
[62] Michael A. Pitts,et al. Early Stages of Figure–Ground Segregation during Perception of the Face–Vase , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[63] Thomas C. Toppino,et al. Enduring interest in perceptual ambiguity: alternating views of reversible figures. , 2004, Psychological bulletin.
[64] C. Blundo,et al. Perception of ambiguous figures after focal brain lesions , 1990, Neuropsychologia.
[65] Michael A. Pitts,et al. Right parietal brain activity precedes perceptual alternation during binocular rivalry , 2011, Human brain mapping.
[66] Thomas C. Toppino,et al. Reversible-figure perception: Mechanisms of intentional control , 2003, Perception & psychophysics.
[67] Michael A. Pitts,et al. Neural generators of ERPs linked with Necker cube reversals. , 2009, Psychophysiology.