Inhibitory control and affective processing in the prefrontal cortex: neuropsychological studies in the common marmoset.

The orbitofrontal cortex has been ascribed a role in the inhibitory control, as well as in the emotional control, of behaviour. While damage to the orbitofrontal cortex in humans and non-human primates can cause inflexibility, impulsiveness and emotional disturbance, the relationship between these effects are unclear. Excitotoxic lesion studies in marmosets comparing the effects of cell loss within specific regions of the prefrontal cortex on performance of a range of behavioural tests reveal that mechanisms of response inhibition are not unique to the orbitofrontal cortex. Instead they are present in distinct cognitive domains for lowerorder as well as higher-order processing throughout the prefrontal cortex. Thus, the lateral prefrontal cortex is involved in the selection and control of action based upon higher-order rules while the orbitofrontal and medial prefrontal cortex may be involved in different but complementary forms of lower-order rule learning, their roles dissociable, as a result of their differential contribution to different types of associative learning.

[1]  Marc D Hauser,et al.  Perseveration, inhibition and the prefrontal cortex: a new look , 1999, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[2]  T. Robbins,et al.  Dissociable Deficits in the Decision-Making Cognition of Chronic Amphetamine Abusers, Opiate Abusers, Patients with Focal Damage to Prefrontal Cortex, and Tryptophan-Depleted Normal Volunteers: Evidence for Monoaminergic Mechanisms , 1999, Neuropsychopharmacology.

[3]  B. Balleine,et al.  Goal-directed instrumental action: contingency and incentive learning and their cortical substrates , 1998, Neuropharmacology.

[4]  H. Damasio,et al.  Dissociation Of Working Memory from Decision Making within the Human Prefrontal Cortex , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[5]  T. Robbins,et al.  Dissociable Forms of Inhibitory Control within Prefrontal Cortex with an Analog of the Wisconsin Card Sort Test: Restriction to Novel Situations and Independence from “On-Line” Processing , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[6]  R. Roth,et al.  Enduring cognitive deficits and cortical dopamine dysfunction in monkeys after long-term administration of phencyclidine. , 1997, Science.

[7]  J. Fuster The Prefrontal Cortex , 1997 .

[8]  M. Raichle,et al.  Subgenual prefrontal cortex abnormalities in mood disorders , 1997, Nature.

[9]  A C Roberts,et al.  Primate analogue of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test: effects of excitotoxic lesions of the prefrontal cortex in the marmoset. , 1996, Behavioral neuroscience.

[10]  E. Rolls,et al.  Face and voice expression identification in patients with emotional and behavioural changes following ventral frontal lobe damage , 1996, Neuropsychologia.

[11]  T. Robbins,et al.  Dissociation in prefrontal cortex of affective and attentional shifts , 1996, Nature.

[12]  A. Damasio,et al.  Failure to respond autonomically to anticipated future outcomes following damage to prefrontal cortex. , 1996, Cerebral cortex.

[13]  C. Gerfen,et al.  The frontal cortex-basal ganglia system in primates. , 1996, Critical reviews in neurobiology.

[14]  T. Preuss Do Rats Have Prefrontal Cortex? The Rose-Woolsey-Akert Program Reconsidered , 1995, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[15]  M. Raichle,et al.  Positron emission tomographic imaging studies of human emotional disorders. , 1995 .

[16]  E. Rolls,et al.  Emotion-related learning in patients with social and emotional changes associated with frontal lobe damage. , 1994, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[17]  M. Petrides Comparative architectonic analysis of the human and the macaque frontal cortex , 1994 .

[18]  J. Schneider,et al.  Delayed matching-to-sample, object retrieval, and discrimination reversal deficits in chronic low dose MPTP-treated monkeys , 1993, Brain Research.

[19]  P. Goldman-Rakic,et al.  Myelo‐ and cytoarchitecture of the granular frontal cortex and surrounding regions in the strepsirhine primate Galago and the anthropoid primate Macaca , 1991, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[20]  T. Shallice,et al.  Deficits in strategy application following frontal lobe damage in man. , 1991, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[21]  A. Damasio,et al.  Somatic markers and the guidance of behavior: Theory and preliminary testing. , 1991 .

[22]  A. Damasio,et al.  Individuals with sociopathic behavior caused by frontal damage fail to respond autonomically to social stimuli , 1990, Behavioural Brain Research.

[23]  A. Diamond Developmental Time Course in Human Infants and Infant Monkeys, and the Neural Bases of, Inhibitory Control in Reaching a , 1990, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[24]  D. Gaffan,et al.  Amygdalar interaction with the mediodorsal nucleus of the thalamus and the ventromedial prefrontal cortex in stimulus-reward associative learning in the monkey , 1990, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[25]  R. Roth,et al.  Cognitive and motor deficits in the acquisition of an object retrieval/detour task in MPTP-treated monkeys. , 1990, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[26]  E. Neafsey,et al.  Prefrontal cortical control of the autonomic nervous system: anatomical and physiological observations. , 1990, Progress in brain research.

[27]  G. Holmes The prefrontal cortex: Anatomy, physiology, and neuropsychology of the frontal lobe (2nd ed.) , 1989 .

[28]  T. Robbins,et al.  The effects of intradimensional and extradimensional shifts on visual discrimination learning in humans and non-human primates , 1988, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. B, Comparative and physiological psychology.

[29]  A. Damasio,et al.  Severe disturbance of higher cognition after bilateral frontal lobe ablation: Patient EVR , 1986 .

[30]  A. Damasio,et al.  Severe disturbance of higher cognition after bilateral frontal lobe ablation , 1985, Neurology.

[31]  K. Brodmann Vergleichende Lokalisationslehre der Großhirnrinde : in ihren Prinzipien dargestellt auf Grund des Zellenbaues , 1985 .

[32]  R. Passingham,et al.  An assessment of the reinforcing properties of foods after amygdaloid lesions in rhesus monkeys. , 1982, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[33]  R. Passingham,et al.  Syndrome produced by lesions of the amygdala in monkeys (Macaca mulatta). , 1981, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[34]  H. D. Steklis,et al.  Effects of orbitofrontal and temporal neocortical lesions on the affiliative behavior of vervet monkeys (Cercopithecus aethiops sabaeus) , 1981, Experimental Neurology.

[35]  J. Fuster Prefrontal Cortex , 2018 .

[36]  R. Myers,et al.  Neural control of social behavior: prefrontal and anterior temporal cortex. , 1973, Neuropsychologia.

[37]  M. Mishkin,et al.  Limbic lesions and the problem of stimulus--reinforcement associations. , 1972, Experimental neurology.

[38]  D. R. Snyder,et al.  Effects of orbital frontal lesions on aversive and aggressive behaviors in rhesus monkeys. , 1970, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[39]  C. Butter,et al.  Perseveration of responding and nonresponding in monkeys with orbital frontal ablations. , 1969, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[40]  Charles M. Butter,et al.  Perseveration in extinction and in discrimination reversal tasks following selective frontal ablations in Macaca mulatta , 1969 .

[41]  William G. Cochran,et al.  Experimental designs, 2nd ed. , 1957 .

[42]  L. Weiskrantz,et al.  Behavioral changes associated with ablation of the amygdaloid complex in monkeys. , 1956, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[43]  William G. Cochran,et al.  Experimental Designs, 2nd Edition , 1950 .

[44]  A. Walker,et al.  A cytoarchitectural study of the prefrontal area of the macaque monkey , 1940 .

[45]  J. Harlow Recovery from the passage of an iron bar through the head , 1993 .