Embodied number processing

concepts become meaningful only when they refer to bodily experiences (e.g., Barsalou, 1999; Fischer & Zwaan, 2008; Glenberg, 1997; Pulvermüller, 2005; Rueschemeyer, Lindemann, van Elk, & Bekkering, 2009). Over the last two decades, research on language comprehension has provided a large amount of evidence for the idea that conceptual knowledge is grounded through such sensory-motor referencing. For instance, reading action-related sentences that imply a particular rotational motion (e.g., she opens the bottle) facilitates the execution of similar actions (e.g., rotating a knob counter-clockwise; Zwaan & Taylor, 2006; see also Glenberg & Kaschak, 2002). This functional link between the motor system and language comprehension has also been found at the neural level, reflected by motor-cortical activation through action-related language processing (e.g., Hauk, Johnsrude, & Pulvermüller, 2004). In turn, there is also evidence for the reverse referencing (in the sense of a functional link) from motor actions onto more efficient processing of action-congruent linguistic descriptions (Rueschemeyer, Lindemann, van Rooij, van Dam, & Bekkering, 2010) and memory retrieval (Pecher, Van Dantzig, Zwaan, & Zeelenberg, 2009; van Dam, Rueschemeyer, Bekkering, & Lindemann, 2013). EMBODIED NUMERICAL COGNITION With this increasing empirical evidence that activation of bodily representations contributes to conceptual knowledge, researchers began to consider the sensory-motor grounding of semantic processing also in the domain of mathematical cognition (e.g., Andres, Olivier, & Badets, 2008; Fischer, 2012; Lakoff & Núñez, 2000; Lindemann, Rueschemeyer, & Bekkering, 2009; Moeller et al., 2012). The present Special Issue presents the latest developments of this approach. The role of sensory and motor codes in number representation and mental arithmetic is important to understand because the two codes share the same class of information, namely knowledge about quantities and magnitudes. Consistent with this fact, similar parietal brain areas support magnitude processing for numbers and for grasping movements (Simon, Mangin, Cohen, Le Bihan, & Dehaene, 2002). Behavioural priming therefore works across the two domains. For example, large numbers facilitate hand opening responses (Andres, Davare, Pesenti, Olivier, & Seron, 2004) and the grasping of objects with a power grip (Lindemann, Abolafia, Girardi, & Bekkering, 2007) whereas small numbers prime hand closure and precision grip actions. In turn, number processing interferes with the timing of the duration of manual button responses (Kiesel & Vierck, 2008) and with the use of a particular response force (Krause, Lindemann, Toni, & Bekkering, 2014). Number processing also impacts judgements about © 2015 Taylor & Francis Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 2015 Vol. 27, No. 4, 381–387, http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/20445911.2015.1032295

[1]  M. H. Fischer,et al.  Cognitive Foundations of Human Number Representations and Mental Arithmetic , 2015 .

[2]  Michael Andres,et al.  Finger-based representation of mental arithmetic , 2015 .

[3]  Martin H. Fischer,et al.  1 + 2 is more than 2 + 1: Violations of commutativity and identity axioms in mental arithmetic , 2015 .

[4]  M. H. Fischer,et al.  Related but not the same: Ordinality, cardinality and 1-to-1 correspondence in finger-based numerical representations , 2015 .

[5]  Giorgio Vallortigara,et al.  At the root of the left–right asymmetries in spatial–numerical processing: From domestic chicks to human subjects , 2015 .

[6]  Dana Ganor-Stern,et al.  Reaching towards an end: Numerical end and distance effects in motor movements , 2015 .

[7]  K. Priftis,et al.  Larger, smaller, odd or even? Task-specific effects of optokinetic stimulation on the mental number space , 2015 .

[8]  U. Cress,et al.  Does your body count? Embodied influences on the preferred counting direction of preschoolers , 2015 .

[9]  Giorgio Vallortigara,et al.  Number-space mapping in the newborn chick resembles humans’ mental number line , 2015, Science.

[10]  K. Moeller,et al.  In touch with numbers: Embodied and situated effects in number magnitude comparison , 2015 .

[11]  Harold Bekkering,et al.  The development of numerosity estimation: Evidence for a linear number representation early in life , 2015 .

[12]  Ann Dowker,et al.  The Oxford Handbook of Numerical Cognition , 2015 .

[13]  Stanley B Klein,et al.  What memory is. , 2015, Wiley interdisciplinary reviews. Cognitive science.

[14]  Martin H. Fischer,et al.  Pushing forward in embodied cognition: may we mouse the mathematical mind? , 2014, Front. Psychol..

[15]  Samuel Shaki,et al.  Spatial Associations in Numerical Cognition—From Single Digits to Arithmetic , 2014, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[16]  Ivan Toni,et al.  Different Brains Process Numbers Differently: Structural Bases of Individual Differences in Spatial and Nonspatial Number Representations , 2014, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[17]  Harold Bekkering,et al.  Spatial Interferences in Mental Arithmetic: Evidence from the Motion–Arithmetic Compatibility Effect , 2014, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[18]  H. Bekkering,et al.  Embodied Grounding of Memory: Toward the Effects of Motor Execution on Memory consolidation , 2013, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[19]  Harold Bekkering,et al.  A Feeling for Numbers: Shared Metric for Symbolic and Tactile Numerosities , 2013, Front. Psychology.

[20]  Ulrike Cress,et al.  Learning and development of embodied numerosity , 2012, Cognitive Processing.

[21]  Martin H. Fischer,et al.  A hierarchical view of grounded, embodied, and situated numerical cognition , 2012, Cognitive Processing.

[22]  B. Bahrami,et al.  Coming of age: A review of embodiment and the neuroscience of semantics , 2012, Cortex.

[23]  V. Natale,et al.  The A Theory Of Magnitude (ATOM) model in temporal perception and reproduction tasks. , 2012, Acta psychologica.

[24]  Valérie Goffaux,et al.  Shifts of spatial attention cued by irrelevant numbers: Electrophysiological evidence from a target discrimination task , 2015 .

[25]  Martin H. Fischer,et al.  The Cultural Number Line: A Review of Cultural and Linguistic Influences on the Development of Number Processing , 2011 .

[26]  Harold Bekkering,et al.  Effects of intentional motor actions on embodied language processing. , 2010, Experimental psychology.

[27]  V Menon,et al.  Functional heterogeneity of inferior parietal cortex during mathematical cognition assessed with cytoarchitectonic probability maps. , 2009, Cerebral cortex.

[28]  Harold Bekkering,et al.  Embodied cognition: The interplay between automatic resonance and selection-for-action mechanisms , 2009 .

[29]  H. Bekkering,et al.  Symbols in numbers: From numerals to magnitude information , 2009 .

[30]  Rolf A. Zwaan,et al.  Short article: Language comprehenders retain implied shape and orientation of objects , 2009, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[31]  Dennis Norris,et al.  Transposed-letter priming of prelexical orthographic representations. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[32]  A. Kiesel,et al.  SNARC-like congruency based on number magnitude and response duration. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[33]  Rolf A. Zwaan,et al.  Embodied Language: A Review of the Role of the Motor System in Language Comprehension , 2008, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[34]  Avishai Henik,et al.  When brightness counts: the neuronal correlate of numerical-luminance interference. , 2008, Cerebral cortex.

[35]  Harold Bekkering,et al.  Getting a grip on numbers: numerical magnitude priming in object grasping. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[36]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Cultural Recycling of Cortical Maps , 2007, Neuron.

[37]  Michael Andres,et al.  Number magnitude potentiates action judgements , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.

[38]  Rolf A. Zwaan,et al.  Seeing, acting, understanding: motor resonance in language comprehension. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[39]  Friedemann Pulvermüller,et al.  Brain mechanisms linking language and action , 2005, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[40]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Interactions between number and space in parietal cortex , 2005, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[41]  Michael Andres,et al.  Number magnitude and grip aperture interaction , 2004, Neuroreport.

[42]  I. Johnsrude,et al.  Somatotopic Representation of Action Words in Human Motor and Premotor Cortex , 2004, Neuron.

[43]  R. Cheloha,et al.  The of a Development , 2004 .

[44]  Vincent Walsh A theory of magnitude: common cortical metrics of time, space and quantity , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[45]  Michael D. Dodd,et al.  Perceiving numbers causes spatial shifts of attention , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.

[46]  Margaret Wilson,et al.  Six views of embodied cognition , 2002, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[47]  Michael P. Kaschak,et al.  Grounding language in action , 2002, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[48]  G. Lakoff,et al.  Where mathematics comes from : how the embodied mind brings mathematics into being , 2002 .

[49]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Topographical Layout of Hand, Eye, Calculation, and Language-Related Areas in the Human Parietal Lobe , 2002, Neuron.

[50]  Klaus Willmes,et al.  Decade breaks in the mental number line? Putting the tens and units back in different bins , 2001, Cognition.

[51]  G. Aschersleben,et al.  The Theory of Event Coding (TEC): a framework for perception and action planning. , 2001, The Behavioral and brain sciences.

[52]  L. Barsalou,et al.  Whither structured representation? , 1999, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[53]  William G. Faris The number sense: How the mind creates mathematics by Stanislas Dehaene , 1998, Complex..

[54]  S. Dehaene,et al.  The Number Sense: How the Mind Creates Mathematics. , 1998 .

[55]  Avishai Henik,et al.  Automatic and intentional processing of numerical information , 1992 .

[56]  J. Gibson The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception , 1979 .

[57]  S. S. Stevens On the psychophysical law. , 1957, Psychological review.