Pragmatic approaches to handling practice effects in longitudinal cognitive aging research

INTRODUCTION The challenge of accounting for practice effects (PEs) when modeling cognitive change was amplified by the COVID-19 pandemic, which introduced period and mode effects that may bias the estimation of cognitive trajectory. METHODS In three Kaiser Permanente Northern California prospective cohorts, we compared predicted cognitive trajectories and the association of grip strength with cognitive decline using three approaches: (1) no acknowledgment of PE, (2) inclusion of a wave indicator, and (3) constraining PE based on a preliminary model (APM) fit using a subset of the data. RESULTS APM-based correction for PEs based on balanced, pre-pandemic data, and with current age as the timescale produced the smallest discrepancy between within-person and between-person estimated age effects. Estimated associations between grip strength and cognitive decline were not sensitive to the approach used. DISCUSSION Constraining PEs based on a preliminary model is a flexible, pragmatic approach allowing for meaningful interpretation of cognitive change. HIGHLIGHTS The magnitude of practice effects (PEs) varied widely by study. When PEs were present, the three PE approaches resulted in divergent estimated age-related cognitive trajectories. Estimated age-related cognitive trajectories were sometimes implausible in models that did not account for PEs. The associations between grip strength and cognitive decline did not differ by the PE approach used. Constraining PEs based on estimates from a preliminary model allows for a meaningful interpretation of cognitive change.

[1]  D. Mungas,et al.  Shifting of Cognitive Assessments Between Face-to-Face and Telephone Administration: Measurement Considerations. , 2022, The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences.

[2]  D. Mungas,et al.  Accounting for lack of representation in dementia research: Generalizing KHANDLE study findings on the prevalence of cognitive impairment to the California older population , 2022, Alzheimer's & dementia : the journal of the Alzheimer's Association.

[3]  M. Bondi,et al.  Cognitive practice effects delay diagnosis of MCI: Implications for clinical trials , 2022, Alzheimer's & dementia.

[4]  M. Bondi,et al.  Accounting for cognitive practice effects results in earlier detection and more accurate diagnosis of MCI: Biomarker confirmation , 2020 .

[5]  D. Suárez-Iglesias,et al.  Physical activity, dietary habits and sleep quality before and during COVID-19 lockdown: A longitudinal study , 2020, Appetite.

[6]  L. Rodríguez-Mañas,et al.  Impact of Social Isolation Due to COVID-19 on Health in Older People: Mental and Physical Effects and Recommendations , 2020, The journal of nutrition, health & aging.

[7]  M. Sliwinski,et al.  Mentally challenging occupations are associated with more rapid cognitive decline at later stages of cognitive aging. , 2019, The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences.

[8]  A. Gross,et al.  DO PEOPLE WITH ALZHEIMER’S DISEASE IMPROVE WITH REPEATED TESTING? UNPACKING THE ROLE OF CONTENT AND CONTEXT IN RETEST EFFECTS , 2017, Alzheimer's & Dementia.

[9]  A. Spiro,et al.  Jump, Hop, or Skip: Modeling Practice Effects in Studies of Determinants of Cognitive Change in Older Adults. , 2016, American journal of epidemiology.

[10]  Carole Dufouil,et al.  Guidelines for reporting methodological challenges and evaluating potential bias in dementia research , 2015, Alzheimer's & Dementia.

[11]  J. Grosch,et al.  Mental work demands, retirement, and longitudinal trajectories of cognitive functioning. , 2014, Journal of occupational health psychology.

[12]  H. Westervelt,et al.  Practitioner’s Guide to Evaluating Change with Neuropsychological Assessment Instruments , 2012, Critical Issues in Neuropsychology.

[13]  D. Tranel,et al.  Scoring Higher the Second Time Around: Meta-Analyses of Practice Effects in Neuropsychological Assessment , 2012, The Clinical neuropsychologist.

[14]  H. Ehrenreich,et al.  Practice effects in healthy adults: A longitudinal study on frequent repetitive cognitive testing , 2010, BMC Neuroscience.

[15]  T. Salthouse Influence of age on practice effects in longitudinal neurocognitive change. , 2010, Neuropsychology.

[16]  Todd Lencz,et al.  Cognitive improvement after treatment with second-generation antipsychotic medications in first-episode schizophrenia: is it a practice effect? , 2007, Archives of general psychiatry.

[17]  E. Siemers,et al.  Practice effects and the use of alternate forms in serial neuropsychological testing. , 2005, Archives of clinical neuropsychology : the official journal of the National Academy of Neuropsychologists.

[18]  Paul K Crane,et al.  Spanish and English Neuropsychological Assessment Scales (SENAS): further development and psychometric characteristics. , 2004, Psychological assessment.

[19]  Emilio Ferrer,et al.  Estimating retest effects in longitudinal assessments of cognitive functioning in adults between 18 and 60 years of age. , 2004, Developmental psychology.

[20]  B. Reed,et al.  Development of psychometrically matched English and Spanish language neuropsychological tests for older persons. , 2000, Neuropsychology.

[21]  S. Blinkhorn,et al.  Developmental Influences On Adult Intelligence , 2005 .