How Do Students Reason About Chemical Substances and Reactions

The central goal of this work is to illustrate how the analysis of chemistry student thinking based on the identification of implicit assumptions about the nature of chemical substances and processes, together with the elicitation of shortcut reasoning strategies used to make judgments and decisions, can help us better explain the difficulties that our students face when asked to use atomic–molecular models of matter to analyze structure–property relationships. This way of conceptualizing student reasoning has several pedagogical advantages. First, it helps us make sense of and bring coherence to a variety of reported alternative conceptions and common student errors in different chemistry topics. Moreover, it facilitates making predictions about students’ ideas and difficulties in many areas. Finally, it provides a framework for analyzing progression of understanding with training in the discipline.

[1]  Vicente Talanquer,et al.  On Cognitive Constraints and Learning Progressions: The case of “structure of matter” , 2009 .

[2]  Jonathan Evans Dual-processing accounts of reasoning, judgment, and social cognition. , 2008, Annual review of psychology.

[3]  Gerd Gigerenzer,et al.  Models of ecological rationality: the recognition heuristic. , 2002, Psychological review.

[4]  Jonathan Evans The heuristic-analytic theory of reasoning: Extension and evaluation , 2006, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[5]  S. Sloman The empirical case for two systems of reasoning. , 1996 .

[6]  D. Kahneman,et al.  Heuristics and Biases: The Psychology of Intuitive Judgment , 2002 .

[7]  A. Woodward,et al.  Learning and the Infant Mind , 2008 .

[8]  Harold H. Harris,et al.  Chemical Misconceptions-Prevention, Diagnosis and Cure; Volume I: Theoretical Background; Volume II: Classroom Resources (Taber, Keith) , 2003 .

[9]  K. Taber,et al.  Coordinating Procedural and Conceptual Knowledge to Make Sense of Word Equations: Understanding the complexity of a ‘simple’ completion task at the learner’s resolution , 2009 .

[10]  Mitchel Resnick,et al.  Turtles, termites, and traffic jams - explorations in massively parallel microworlds , 1994 .

[11]  J. Shea National Science Education Standards , 1995 .

[12]  Keith S. Taber,et al.  An alternative conceptual framework from chemistry education , 1998 .

[13]  B. Andersson,et al.  The experiential gestalt of causation: a common core to pupils’ preconceptions in science , 1986 .

[14]  Frank C. Keil,et al.  Constraints on Constraints: Surveying the Epigenetic Landscape , 1990, Cogn. Sci..

[15]  A. Ortony,et al.  Similarity and Analogical Reasoning , 1991 .

[16]  Katherine D. Kinzler,et al.  Core knowledge. , 2007, Developmental science.

[17]  College Students’ Conceptions of Chemical Stability: The widespread adoption of a heuristic rule out of context and beyond its range of application , 2009 .

[18]  Tina A. Grotzer Learning to Understand the Forms of Causality Implicit in Scientifically Accepted Explanations , 2003 .

[19]  Vicente A Talanquer,et al.  Students' predictions about the sensory properties of chemical compounds: Additive versus emergent frameworks , 2008 .

[20]  Uri Leron,et al.  The Rationality Debate: Application of Cognitive Psychology to Mathematics Education , 2006 .

[21]  U. Wilensky,et al.  Complex Systems in Education: Scientific and Educational Importance and Implications for the Learning Sciences , 2006 .

[22]  S. Vosniadou Capturing and modeling the process of conceptual change. , 1994 .

[23]  Stella Vosniadou,et al.  International handbook of research on conceptual change , 2013 .

[24]  V. Talanquer,et al.  Heuristic Reasoning in Chemistry: Making decisions about acid strength , 2011 .

[25]  James D. Slotta,et al.  Helping Students Understand Challenging Topics in Science Through Ontology Training , 2006 .

[26]  S. Gelman Learning from others: children's construction of concepts. , 2009, Annual review of psychology.

[27]  Laurence Viennot Reasoning in Physics: The Part of Common Sense , 2001 .

[28]  Edward F. Redish,et al.  A Theoretical Framework for Physics Education Research: Modeling student thinking , 2004 .

[29]  S. Vosniadou The Framework Theory Approach to the Problem of Conceptual Change , 2009 .

[30]  R. Baillargeon,et al.  An Account of Infants' Physical Reasoning , 2008 .

[31]  A. diSessa,et al.  What changes in conceptual change , 1998 .

[32]  J. Gilbert Chemical education: towards research-based practice , 2003 .

[33]  P M Todd,et al.  Précis of Simple heuristics that make us smart , 2000, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[34]  M. Chi,et al.  Naive Physics Reasoning: A Commitment to Substance-Based Conceptions , 2000 .

[35]  M. Nakhleh Why some students don't learn chemistry: Chemical misconceptions , 1992 .

[36]  W. McComas Benchmarks for Science Literacy , 2014 .

[37]  Keith S. Taber,et al.  Learning Processes in Chemistry: Drawing Upon Cognitive Resources to Learn About the Particulate Structure of Matter , 2010 .

[38]  Vicente A Talanquer,et al.  Commonsense Chemistry: A Model for Understanding Students' Alternative Conceptions , 2006 .

[39]  Ruth Stavy,et al.  How Students Mis/Understand Science and Mathematics: Intuitive Rules (Ways of Knowing in Science Series) , 2000 .

[40]  Vicente A Talanquer,et al.  The role of intuitive heuristics in students' thinking: Ranking chemical substances , 2010 .

[41]  Brian F. Bowdle,et al.  The career of metaphor. , 2005, Psychological review.

[42]  A. diSessa Toward an Epistemology of Physics , 1993 .

[43]  Vicente A Talanquer,et al.  Explanations and Teleology in Chemistry Education , 2007 .

[44]  G. Hatano,et al.  Domain-specific constraints of conceptual development , 2000 .

[45]  David Hammer,et al.  Conceptual change in physics , 2013 .

[46]  Vicente A Talanquer,et al.  Exploring Dominant Types of Explanations Built by General Chemistry Students , 2010 .

[47]  S. Hannabuss The Stuff of Thought: Language as a Window into Human Nature , 2008 .