Variations in pattern of pubertal changes in girls.

In Great Britain some girls begin to menstruate in their 10th or 11th years, while others who are equally healthy may not experience menarche until they 'are 14, 15, or even older. Menarche is, however, only a single event in the combination of physical changes which constitute puberty. The adolescent growth spurt, the development of the breasts, and the growth of the pubic hair occur more or less concurrently, and take, on the average, about 3 years from beginning to completion, with menarche occurring usually in the latter half of this period (Tanner, 1962). At present we lack detailed information about the rate at which girls progress through the stages of puberty and about the relation of one event to another. Only longitudinal studies (i.e. studies in which the same individuals are examined repeatedly over a period of time) can provide this information, which would be helpful both to the clinician in distinguishing the normal from the abnormal, and to the neuro-endocrinologist in constructing hypotheses about the mechanisms by which puberty is controlled. Present knowledge is based on studies carried out on small numbers of children in the United States a generation ago, together with some German studies of a similar period (for literature see Tanner, 1962). The only recent European study in which the events of puberty have been followed longitudinally is the Harpenden Growth Study, which began 19 years ago in England and is still in progress. Anthropometric measurements have been taken, and the development of the breasts and pubic hair have been recorded photographically at 3monthly intervals throughout puberty. Though these data may be subject to certain biases, discussed below, they provide information available from no other source. This paper describes the extent of normal individual variation observed in the events of puberty among the girls of the Harpenden Growth Study. We discuss: (a) variation in the chrono-

[1]  A. Swan,et al.  Computing Maximum-likelihood Estimates for Parameters of the Normal Distribution from Grouped and Censored Data , 1969 .

[2]  W. A. Marshall,et al.  Growth and physiological development during adolescence. , 1968, Annual review of medicine.

[3]  J M Tanner,et al.  Standards from birth to maturity for height, weight, height velocity, and weight velocity: British children, 1965. II. , 1966, Archives of disease in childhood.

[4]  J. Tanner,et al.  Standards from birth to maturity for height, weight, height velocity, and weight velocity: British children, 1965. I. , 1966, Archives of disease in childhood.

[5]  K. Chaudhuri The Institute of Child Health , 1957, The Lancet.

[6]  M. M. Lee,et al.  SEXUAL MATURATION OF CHINESE GIRLS IN HONG KONG. , 1963, Pediatrics.

[7]  J. A. Scott Report of the Heights and Weights (and Other Measurements) of School Pupils in the County of London in 1959 , 1961 .

[8]  J. Deming Application of the Gompertz curve to the observed pattern of growth in length of 48 individual boys and girls during the adolescent cycle of growth. , 1957, Human biology.

[9]  D. A. Sholl,et al.  Growth at Adolescence , 1962 .

[10]  C. Hanley,et al.  Indices of physiological maturity: derivation and interrelationships. , 1953, Child development.

[11]  J. Waterhouse,et al.  Studies on Puberty , 1949 .

[12]  E. L. Reynolds,et al.  Individual differences in physical changes associated with adolescence in girls. , 1948, American journal of diseases of children.

[13]  L. Hogben,et al.  Studies on Puberty. Part I , 1948 .

[14]  H. Elftman,et al.  SEX DIFFERENCES IN PUBIC HAIR DISTRIBUTION , 1946 .

[15]  F. K. Shuttleworth The physical and mental growth of girls and boys age six to nineteen in relation to age at maximum growth , 1939 .