The Creative Experiences Questionnaire (CEQ): A brief self-report measure of fantasy proneness

The current article describes the psychometric qualities of the Creative Experiences Questionnaire (CEQ), a brief 25-item self-report measure of fantasy proneness. Findings indicate that the CEQ demonstrates adequate test-retest stability and internal consistency. CEQ scores appear not to be related to social desirability. The CEQ was found to be strongly correlated with a concurrent measure of fantasy proneness. Furthermore, there are substantial correlations between the CEQ and standard measures of absorption, schizotypy, and dissociation. Bearing in mind that these constructs are thought to be intimately linked to fantasy proneness, this pattern of correlations supports the validity of the CEQ. The CEQ might be fruitfully used as a brief research scale in several domains (e.g. studies on pseudomemories).

[1]  I. Kirsch,et al.  Interpretive sets, expectancy, fantasy proneness, and dissociation as predictors of hypnotic response. , 1992, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[2]  Elizabeth F. Loftus,et al.  Imagination inflation: Imagining a childhood event inflates confidence that it occurred , 1996, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[3]  Sebastian Weisdorf,et al.  Becoming Famous Overnight: Limits on the Ability to Avoid Unconscious Influences of the Past , 2001 .

[4]  Irving Kirsch,et al.  Imaginative Suggestibility and Hypnotizability , 2001 .

[5]  T. Barber,et al.  Vivid Fantasy and Hallucinatory Abilities in the Life Histories of Excellent Hypnotic Subjects (“Somnambules”): Preliminary Report with Female Subjects , 1981 .

[6]  M. Eysenck,et al.  Personality and Individual Differences: A Natural Science Approach , 1985 .

[7]  S. Lynn,et al.  Fantasy proneness, DSM-III-R axis I psychopathology, and dissociation. , 1995, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[8]  L. Jacoby,et al.  Becoming Famous Overnight: Limits on the Ability to Avoid Unconscious Influences of the Past , 1989 .

[9]  H. Schmidt,et al.  De Creatieve Ervaringen Vragenlijst als maat voor 'fantasy proneness' , 1998 .

[10]  A. Tellegen,et al.  Openness to absorbing and self-altering experiences ("absorption"), a trait related to hypnotic susceptibility. , 1974, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[11]  K. Dickson,et al.  Close encounters: an examination of UFO experiences. , 1993, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[12]  Harald Merckelbach,et al.  Dissociative experiences and interrogative suggestibility in college students , 2000 .

[13]  J. Kihlstrom,et al.  Dissociative tendencies and dissociative disorders. , 1994, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[14]  John A. Johnson,et al.  Handbook of personality psychology. , 1997 .

[15]  I. Kirsch,et al.  Imaginative suggestibility and hypnotizability: an empirical analysis. , 1999, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[16]  E. Bernstein,et al.  Development, reliability, and validity of a dissociation scale. , 1986, The Journal of nervous and mental disease.

[17]  Individual Differences and the C reation of False C hildhood M emories , 1998 .

[18]  S. C. Lacey,et al.  Absorption, dissociation, and fantasy-proneness as predictors of memory distortion in autobiographical and laboratory-generated memories , 1998 .

[19]  A. Raine The SPQ: a scale for the assessment of schizotypal personality based on DSM-III-R criteria. , 1991, Schizophrenia bulletin.

[20]  J. Saver,et al.  The neural substrates of religious experience. , 1997, The Journal of neuropsychiatry and clinical neurosciences.

[21]  S. Lynn,et al.  Fantasy Proneness: Developmental Antecedents , 1987 .

[22]  R. Horselenberg,et al.  Dissociative experiences, response bias, and fantasy proneness in college students , 2000 .

[23]  P. Costa,et al.  Conceptions and correlates of openness to experience. , 1997 .

[24]  H. Merckelbach,et al.  Fantasy proneness and cognitive failures as correlates of dissociative experiences , 1999 .

[25]  N. Kerr,et al.  Imagery: Current Theory, Research, and Application , 1985 .

[26]  R. Horselenberg,et al.  Imagining fictitious childhood events: the role of individual differences in imagination inflation , 2000 .

[27]  Fantasy proneness, DSM-III-R axis I psychopathology, and dissociation. , 1995 .

[28]  J. G. Allen,et al.  Dissociation and vulnerability to psychotic experience. The Dissociative Experiences Scale and the MMPI-2. , 1995, The Journal of nervous and mental disease.

[29]  J. Nicholls,et al.  Some dangers of using personality questionnaires to study personality. , 1982 .

[30]  S. Lynn,et al.  Fantasy proneness. Hypnosis, developmental antecedents, and psychopathology. , 1988, The American psychologist.

[31]  CLINICAL ASSESSMENT OF MALINGERING AND DECEPTION , 1990 .

[32]  G. Claridge,et al.  Schizotypy and hemisphere function—I: Theoretical considerations and the measurement of schizotypy , 1984 .

[33]  N. Barraclough,et al.  Modelling childhood causes of paranormal belief and experience: Childhood trauma and childhood fantasy , 1995 .

[34]  R. Rogers Clinical assessment of malingering and deception , 1988 .

[35]  R. Wiers,et al.  Effects of retrieving childhood events on metamemory judgments depend on the questions you ask. , 2001, The British journal of clinical psychology.

[36]  T. Barber,et al.  The fantasy-prone personality: Implications for understanding imagery, hypnosis, and parapsychological phenomena. , 1982 .

[37]  H. Merckelbach,et al.  Dissociation, schizotypy, and fantasy proneness in undergraduate students. , 2000, The Journal of nervous and mental disease.

[38]  I. Hyman,et al.  Individual differences and the creation of false childhood memories. , 1998, Memory.