The evolution of anticipatory cognition as a precursor to symbolic communication

This article proposes an ecologically based answer to why humans are the only animals who have developed a symbolic communication system. The first part of the argument claims that the Oldowan culture generated selective forces that lead to the evolution of anticipatory cognition, that is, the ability to mentally represent future needs and events. We argue that anticipatory cognition opened up for new forms of cooperation about future goals that were beneficial for hominid societies. Symbolic communication then emerged as the most efficient way of solving problems concerning cooperation about future non-existent goals. 1. What are the significant forces behind the evolution of language? Explaining how language evolved involves answering two quite distinct scientific questions (Bickerton 2003, Tomasello 2003). The first is why humans have developed a system of symbolic representation as a basis for much of their communication. The second is why this system has acquired the structural characteristics of the syntax of extant human languages. In this paper, we focus on the first question. Homo sapiens is the only species with a symbolic language. If one believes that language has come into existence according to the principles of evolutionary theory, there should be some selective advantage that has fostered the development of language among humans. Recently, a popular approach has been that language arose as a result of increased social interaction, for example as a consequence of increasing group size (Dunbar 1996) or as some form of ritualization (Deacon, 1997; Knight, 1998). However, despite all the merits of these proposals, they have problems explaining why language has not evolved among other apes or animals, at least to some degree (Bickerton, 2002, p. 209; Gärdenfors, 2004, p. 237). As a matter of fact, the social interaction among many species is very strong. If social interaction is the crucial reason for the evolution of language, it should be expected that at least some other primate species would have developed some communication forms that go beyond animal call systems. Bickerton (2002, p. 210) concludes: “When a complex and unique development occurs in only one species, the most logical conclusion is that the selective pressure driving that development must have been unique to that species. Thus the strength of social intelligence in other primates argues against, not for, social intelligence as the force behind the emergence of language.” The upshot is that some other evolutionary forces behind the evolution of language must be identified – forces that have only applied to the hominid line. One factor that has been surprisingly neglected in the discussions of the evolution of language is the difference in the ecology of the early hominids and the other apes (Bickerton 2002). Following Osvath and

[1]  Robin I. M. Dunbar The thinking ape: Evolutionary origins of intelligence , 1995 .

[2]  E. Vrba,et al.  Environment and behavior of 2.5-million-year-old Bouri hominids. , 1999, Science.

[3]  N. Toth,et al.  Microwear polishes on early stone tools from Koobi Fora, Kenya , 1981, Nature.

[4]  Scott Atran,et al.  In Gods We Trust: The Evolutionary Landscape of Religion , 2005 .

[5]  M. Winterbottom,et al.  How Homo became Sapiens — On the Evolution of Thinking , 2006 .

[6]  M. Hauser Wild Minds: What Animals Really Think , 2000 .

[7]  Michael J. Rogers,et al.  2.6-Million-year-old stone tools and associated bones from OGS-6 and OGS-7, Gona, Afar, Ethiopia. , 2003, Journal of human evolution.

[8]  J. Gomez Apes, Monkeys, Children, and the Growth of Mind , 2004 .

[9]  M. Tomasello On the Different Origins of Symbols and Grammar , 2003 .

[10]  T. Plummer Flaked stones and old bones: biological and cultural evolution at the dawn of technology. , 2004, American journal of physical anthropology.

[11]  Peter Gärdenfors,et al.  Cooperation in apes and humans , 2003 .

[12]  Mathias Osvath,et al.  Oldowan culture and the evolution of anticipatory cognition , 2005 .

[13]  C. J. Cela-Conde,et al.  On the phylogeny of human morality (ten years later) , 1990 .

[14]  H. Roitblat The meaning of representation in animal memory , 1982, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[15]  Robin I. M. Dunbar Grooming, Gossip and the Evolution of Language , 1996 .

[16]  W. Leonard,et al.  Ecological correlates of home range variation in primates: Implications for hominid evolution , 2000 .

[17]  M. Donald Origins of the modern mind , 1991 .

[18]  Doris Bischof-Köhler,et al.  Zur Phylogenese menschlicher Motivation , 1985 .

[19]  B L Schwartz,et al.  Episodic memory in primates , 2001, American journal of primatology.

[20]  C. F. Hockett The origin of speech. , 1960, Scientific American.

[21]  W. Leonard,et al.  Comparative primate energetics and hominid evolution. , 1997, American journal of physical anthropology.

[22]  R. Byrne,et al.  Why creative intelligence is hard to find , 1988, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[23]  Peter Giidenfors Cued and detached representations in animal cognition , 1996 .

[24]  J. Russell,et al.  Evidence for executive dysfunction in autism , 1994, Neuropsychologia.

[25]  N. Toth The oldowan reassessed: a close look at early stone artifacts , 1985 .

[26]  M. Corballis,et al.  Mental time travel and the evolution of the human mind. , 1997, Genetic, social, and general psychology monographs.

[27]  J. Stevenson The cultural origins of human cognition , 2001 .

[28]  P. Gärdenfors,et al.  Bodily mimesisas “the missing link” in human cognitive evolution , 2005 .

[29]  Chris Knight,et al.  5 Ritual/speech coevolution: a solution to the problem of deception , 1998 .

[30]  Thomas Suddendorf,et al.  Mental time travel in animals? , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[31]  C. Thinus-Blanc,et al.  Cognitive Processes and Spatial Orientation in Animal and Man , 1987 .

[32]  Derek Bickerton,et al.  Symbol and Structure: A Comprehensive Framework for Language Evolution , 2003 .

[33]  D. Bickerton Language and Species , 1990 .

[34]  P. Gärdenfors Cooperation and the evolution of symbolic communication , 2004 .

[35]  Kim Hill,et al.  A theory of human life history evolution: Diet, intelligence, and longevity , 2000 .

[36]  Rick Grush,et al.  The architecture of representation , 1997 .

[37]  Jacques Vauclair,et al.  “Language” and intelligence in monkeys and apes: Primate cognition: From representation to language , 1990 .

[38]  A. Glenberg,et al.  What memory is for: Creating meaning in the service of action , 1997, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[39]  E. Tulving How many memory systems are there , 1985 .