Temperamental exuberance and executive function predict propensity for risk taking in childhood

Abstract The present study utilized a multilevel approach to examine developmental trajectories in risk-taking propensity. We examined the moderating role of specific executive function components, attention shifting and inhibitory control, on the link between exuberant temperament in infancy and propensity for risk taking in childhood. Risk taking was assessed using a task previously associated with sensation seeking and antisocial behaviors. Two hundred ninety-one infants were brought into the lab and behaviors reflecting exuberance were observed at 4, 9, 24, and 36 months of age. Executive function was assessed at 48 months of age. Risk-taking propensity was measured when children were 60 months of age. The results indicated that exuberance and attention shifting, but not inhibitory control, significantly interacted to predict propensity for risk taking. Exuberance was positively associated with risk-taking propensity among children who were relatively low in attention shifting but unrelated for children high in attention shifting. These findings illustrated the multifinality of developmental outcomes for temperamentally exuberant young children and pointed to the distinct regulatory influences of different executive functions for children of differing temperaments. Attention shifting likely affords a child the ability to consider both positive and negative consequences and moderates the relation between early exuberance and risk-taking propensity.

[1]  N. Fox,et al.  Temperamental exuberance and executive function predict propensity for risk taking in childhood—CORRIGENDUM , 2013, Development and Psychopathology.

[2]  M. Gunnar,et al.  Temperamental Exuberance: Correlates and Consequences , 2012 .

[3]  N. Fox,et al.  Behavioural inhibition: Is it a risk factor for anxiety? , 2011, International review of psychiatry.

[4]  N. Fox,et al.  Longitudinal stability of temperamental exuberance and social-emotional outcomes in early childhood. , 2011, Developmental psychology.

[5]  L. Steinberg,et al.  Peers increase adolescent risk taking by enhancing activity in the brain's reward circuitry. , 2011, Developmental science.

[6]  Behavioral Inhibition and Anxiety: The Moderating Roles of Inhibitory Control and Attention Shifting , 2011, Journal of abnormal child psychology.

[7]  David M. Sobel,et al.  Children's beliefs about the fantasy/reality status of hypothesized machines. , 2011, Developmental science.

[8]  Elizabeth K. Reynolds,et al.  Early temperament, propensity for risk-taking and adolescent substance-related problems: a prospective multi-method investigation. , 2010, Addictive behaviors.

[9]  Elizabeth K. Reynolds,et al.  Changes in sensation seeking and risk-taking propensity predict increases in alcohol use among early adolescents. , 2010, Alcoholism, clinical and experimental research.

[10]  L. Somerville,et al.  Developmental neurobiology of cognitive control and motivational systems , 2010, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[11]  E. Claus,et al.  Age differences in affective decision making as indexed by performance on the Iowa Gambling Task. , 2010, Developmental psychology.

[12]  H. Dewit,et al.  Risk taking differences on a behavioral task as a function of potential reward/loss magnitude and individual differences in impulsivity and sensation seeking , 2009, Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior.

[13]  D. Cicchetti,et al.  Child maltreatment and trajectories of personality and behavioral functioning: Implications for the development of personality disorder , 2009, Development and Psychopathology.

[14]  J. Arnett,et al.  Adolescence and Emerging Adulthood: A Cultural Approach , 2009 .

[15]  E. Weber,et al.  Affective and deliberative processes in risky choice: age differences in risk taking in the Columbia Card Task. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[16]  M. Vasey,et al.  Negative Affectivity, Effortful Control, and Attention to Threat-Relevant Stimuli , 2009, Journal of abnormal child psychology.

[17]  Monique Ernst,et al.  Neuroimaging in Developmental Clinical Neuroscience , 2009 .

[18]  Eveline A. Crone,et al.  Neuroimaging in Developmental Clinical Neuroscience: Neural correlates of the development of cognitive control , 2009 .

[19]  D. Cicchetti,et al.  The past achievements and future promises of developmental psychopathology: the coming of age of a discipline. , 2009, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[20]  Tara L. White,et al.  Test-retest characteristics of the Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART). , 2008, Experimental and clinical psychopharmacology.

[21]  N. Fox,et al.  Behavioral reactivity and approach-withdrawal bias in infancy. , 2008, Developmental psychology.

[22]  Laudan B. Jahromi,et al.  Exuberant and inhibited toddlers: Stability of temperament and risk for problem behavior , 2008, Development and Psychopathology.

[23]  Stephanie M. Carlson,et al.  Development of executive function during childhood , 2016 .

[24]  H. de Wit,et al.  Personality and gender differences in effects of d-amphetamine on risk taking. , 2007, Experimental and clinical psychopharmacology.

[25]  Gary Glover,et al.  Frontostriatal connectivity and its role in cognitive control in parent-child dyads with ADHD. , 2007, The American journal of psychiatry.

[26]  N. Fox,et al.  Printed in the United States of America DOI: 10.1017/S0954579407000363 Behavioral inhibition and anxiety disorders: Multiple levels of a resilience process , 2022 .

[27]  Stacey B. Daughters,et al.  Reliability and Validity of the Youth Version of the Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART–Y) in the Assessment of Risk-Taking Behavior Among Inner-City Adolescents , 2007, Journal of clinical child and adolescent psychology : the official journal for the Society of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, American Psychological Association, Division 53.

[28]  D. Cicchetti,et al.  Toward the application of a multiple-levels-of-analysis perspective to research in development and psychopathology , 2007 .

[29]  C. Liston,et al.  Frontostriatal microstructure modulates efficient recruitment of cognitive control. , 2006, Cerebral cortex.

[30]  J. Allen,et al.  Leaders and followers in adolescent close friendships: Susceptibility to peer influence as a predictor of risky behavior, friendship instability, and depression , 2006, Development and Psychopathology.

[31]  Philip David Zelazo,et al.  The Dimensional Change Card Sort (DCCS): a method of assessing executive function in children , 2006, Nature Protocols.

[32]  E. Butelman,et al.  Genetic influences on impulsivity, risk taking, stress responsivity and vulnerability to drug abuse and addiction , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.

[33]  Timothy J. Pleskac,et al.  Modeling behavior in a clinically diagnostic sequential risk-taking task. , 2005, Psychological review.

[34]  N. Fox,et al.  Temperament and anxiety disorders. , 2005, Child and adolescent psychiatric clinics of North America.

[35]  C. Lejuez,et al.  Differences in risk-taking propensity across inner-city adolescent ever- and never-smokers. , 2005, Nicotine & tobacco research : official journal of the Society for Research on Nicotine and Tobacco.

[36]  N. Fox,et al.  A Behavioral and Electrophysiological Study of Children's Selective Attention Under Neutral and Affective Conditions , 2005 .

[37]  C. Stifter,et al.  Behavioral approach-inhibition in toddlers: prediction from infancy, positive and negative affective components, and relations with behavior problems. , 2005, Child development.

[38]  R. M. Tobin,et al.  Effortful Control and Children's Responses to Undesirable Gifts , 2005 .

[39]  Tracy L. Spinrad,et al.  The relations of problem behavior status to children's negative emotionality, effortful control, and impulsivity: concurrent relations and prediction of change. , 2005, Developmental psychology.

[40]  K. E. Nichols,et al.  Behavioral inhibition: linking biology and behavior within a developmental framework. , 2005, Annual review of psychology.

[41]  M. Farah,et al.  Neurocognitive correlates of socioeconomic status in kindergarten children. , 2005, Developmental science.

[42]  M. Posner,et al.  Developing mechanisms of temperamental effortful control. , 2003, Journal of personality.

[43]  Josef Perner,et al.  Training transfer between card sorting and false belief understanding: helping children apply conflicting descriptions. , 2003, Child development.

[44]  Will M Aklin,et al.  Evaluation of the Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART) as a predictor of adolescent real-world risk-taking behaviours. , 2003, Journal of adolescence.

[45]  Erika S. Trapl,et al.  Parental monitoring, negotiated unsupervised time, and parental trust: the role of perceived parenting practices in adolescent health risk behaviors. , 2003, The Journal of adolescent health : official publication of the Society for Adolescent Medicine.

[46]  Avshalom Caspi,et al.  Children's behavioral styles at age 3 are linked to their adult personality traits at age 26. , 2003, Journal of personality.

[47]  Cindy A. Crusto,et al.  What works in prevention. Principles of effective prevention programs. , 2003, The American psychologist.

[48]  Will M Aklin,et al.  The Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART) differentiates smokers and nonsmokers. , 2003, Experimental and clinical psychopharmacology.

[49]  F. Crawford,et al.  DRD4 and novelty seeking: results of meta-analyses. , 2002, American journal of medical genetics.

[50]  Gregory L. Stuart,et al.  Evaluation of a behavioral measure of risk taking: the Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART). , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Applied.

[51]  R. Davidson,et al.  Continuity and change in inhibited and uninhibited children. , 2002, Child development.

[52]  J. Lusher,et al.  Dopamine D4 receptor gene (DRD4) is associated with Novelty Seeking (NS) and substance abuse: the saga continues . . . , 2001, Molecular Psychiatry.

[53]  L. Moses,et al.  Individual differences in inhibitory control and children's theory of mind. , 2001, Child development.

[54]  Kenneth H. Rubin,et al.  Continuity and discontinuity of behavioral inhibition and exuberance: psychophysiological and behavioral influences across the first four years of life. , 2001, Child development.

[55]  C. Nelson,et al.  Handbook of Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience , 2001 .

[56]  S. Dowsett,et al.  The development of inhibitory control in preschool children: effects of "executive skills" training. , 2000, Developmental psychobiology.

[57]  R. Depue,et al.  Neurobiology of the structure of personality: Dopamine, facilitation of incentive motivation, and extraversion , 1999, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[58]  B. Leigh Peril, chance, adventure: concepts of risk, alcohol use and risky behavior in young adults. , 1999, Addiction.

[59]  J. Panksepp Affective Neuroscience: The Foundations of Human and Animal Emotions , 1998 .

[60]  J. Belsky,et al.  Infant emotionality, parenting, and 3-year inhibition: exploring stability and lawful discontinuity in a male sample. , 1997, Developmental psychology.

[61]  N. Eisenberg,et al.  The relations of regulation and emotionality to problem behavior in elementary school children , 1996, Development and Psychopathology.

[62]  D L Newman,et al.  Behavioral observations at age 3 years predict adult psychiatric disorders. Longitudinal evidence from a birth cohort. , 1996, Archives of general psychiatry.

[63]  Dante Cicchetti,et al.  Equifinality and multifinality in developmental psychopathology , 1996, Development and Psychopathology.

[64]  N. Fox,et al.  Behavioral and physiological antecedents of inhibited and uninhibited behavior. , 1996, Child development.

[65]  William B. Hansen,et al.  Handbook of adolescent health risk behavior. , 1996 .

[66]  G. Kochanska Children's Temperament, Mothers' Discipline, and Security of Attachment: Multiple Pathways to Emerging Internalization , 1995 .

[67]  A. Diamond,et al.  The relationship between cognition and action: performance of children 3 1 2 –7 years old on a stroop- like day-night test , 1994, Cognition.

[68]  M. Rothbart,et al.  Temperament and social behavior in childhood , 1994 .

[69]  Larry E. Toothaker,et al.  Multiple Regression: Testing and Interpreting Interactions , 1991 .

[70]  T. Moffitt,et al.  Explaining the relation between IQ and delinquency: class, race, test motivation, school failure, or self-control? , 1993, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[71]  S. Guze Psychobiology of Personality , 1992 .

[72]  J. Kagan,et al.  Temperamental factors in human development. , 1991, The American psychologist.

[73]  M. Zuckerman,et al.  Influences of sensation seeking, gender, risk appraisal, and situational motivation on smoking. , 1990, Addictive behaviors.

[74]  J. Kagan,et al.  Biological bases of childhood shyness. , 1988, Science.

[75]  Kagan,et al.  The physiology and psychology of behavioral inhibition in children. , 1987, Child development.

[76]  J. Gray,et al.  The neuropsychology of anxiety. , 1985, Issues in mental health nursing.

[77]  J. Gray,et al.  Précis of The neuropsychology of anxiety: An enquiry into the functions of the septo-hippocampal system , 1982, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[78]  Barbara K. Keogh,et al.  Temperament , 1982 .

[79]  W. Gibson Three multivariate models: Factor analysis, latent structure analysis, and latent profile analysis , 1959 .