Discerning nonrigid 3D shapes from motion cues
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] D. Proffitt,et al. Depth perception in motion parallax and stereokinesis. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[2] J. Allman,et al. Stimulus specific responses from beyond the classical receptive field: neurophysiological mechanisms for local-global comparisons in visual neurons. , 1985, Annual review of neuroscience.
[3] D D Hoffman,et al. Inferring Structure from Motion in Two-View and Multiview Displays , 1993, Perception.
[4] R A Andersen,et al. The response of area MT and V1 neurons to transparent motion , 1991, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.
[5] Ellen C. Hildreth,et al. Measurement of Visual Motion , 1984 .
[6] H. C. Longuet-Higgins,et al. The interpretation of a moving retinal image , 1980, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B. Biological Sciences.
[7] R. Andersen,et al. Response of MSTd neurons to simulated 3D orientation of rotating planes. , 2002, Journal of neurophysiology.
[8] Tim S. Meese,et al. Speed gradients and the perception of surface slant: Analysis is two-dimensional not one-dimensional , 1995, Vision Research.
[9] S. Ullman. The interpretation of structure from motion , 1979, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B. Biological Sciences.
[10] K. Nakayama,et al. Single visual neurons code opposing motion independent of direction. , 1983, Science.
[11] Gregory J Zelinsky,et al. The role of "rescue saccades" in tracking objects through occlusions. , 2010, Journal of vision.
[12] F. Domini,et al. 3-D structure perceived from dynamic information: a new theory , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[13] J J Koenderink,et al. Depth and shape from differential perspective in the presence of bending deformations. , 1986, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.
[14] J T Todd,et al. The perception of 3-dimensional affine structure from minimal apparent motion sequences , 1990, Perception & psychophysics.
[15] G Sperling,et al. Kinetic depth effect and identification of shape. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[16] M Nawrot,et al. A neural network model of kinetic depth , 1991, Visual Neuroscience.
[17] Keiji Tanaka,et al. Integration of direction signals of image motion in the superior temporal sulcus of the macaque monkey , 1986, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.
[18] K. Nakayama,et al. A Velocity Analogue of Brightness Contrast , 1973, Perception.
[19] Hermann von Helmholtz,et al. Treatise on Physiological Optics , 1962 .
[20] F. Domini,et al. Recovery of 3-D structure from motion is neither euclidean nor affine , 1998 .
[21] A. Watson,et al. Quest: A Bayesian adaptive psychometric method , 1983, Perception & psychophysics.
[22] J. Gibson,et al. Parallax and perspective during aircraft landings. , 1955, The American journal of psychology.
[23] J. Marsh,et al. Anisotropies in the Perception of Three-Dimensional Surfaces , .
[24] G Sperling,et al. How to study the kinetic depth effect experimentally. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[25] C Caudek,et al. Perceiving surface slant from deformation of optic flow. , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[26] Bart Farell,et al. A new theory of structure-from-motion perception. , 2009, Journal of vision.
[27] T. Poggio,et al. Cognitive neuroscience: Neural mechanisms for the recognition of biological movements , 2003, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[28] B. Rogers,et al. Simultaneous and Successive Contrast Effects in the Perception of Depth from Motion-Parallax and Stereoscopic Information , 1982, Perception.
[29] Roberto Cipolla,et al. Robust structure from motion using motion parallax , 1993, 1993 (4th) International Conference on Computer Vision.
[30] A. Fitzgibbon,et al. Humans Ignore Motion and Stereo Cues in Favor of a Fictional Stable World , 2006, Current Biology.
[31] A. Yuille,et al. The perceived motion of a stereokinetic stimulus , 2006, Vision Research.
[32] Norberto M. Grzywacz,et al. A computational theory for the perception of coherent visual motion , 1988, Nature.
[33] Takeo Watanabe,et al. 3D surface perception from motion involves a temporal–parietal network , 2009, The European journal of neuroscience.
[34] K. Tanaka,et al. Analysis of motion of the visual field by direction, expansion/contraction, and rotation cells clustered in the dorsal part of the medial superior temporal area of the macaque monkey. , 1989, Journal of neurophysiology.
[35] F. Scharnowski,et al. Long-lasting modulation of feature integration by transcranial magnetic stimulation. , 2009, Journal of vision.
[36] J. Aloimonos,et al. On the kinetic depth effect , 1989, Biological Cybernetics.
[37] Jan J. Koenderink,et al. Local structure of movement parallax of the plane , 1976 .
[38] M. Hogervorst,et al. The role of perspective effects and accelerations in perceived three-dimensional structure-from-motion. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[39] Brendon O. Watson,et al. Computing relief structure from motion with a distributed velocity and disparity representation , 2002, Vision Research.
[40] A. T. Smith,et al. On the sensitivity of complex cells in feline striate cortex to relative motion , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.
[41] Q. Zaidi,et al. The utility of shape attributes in deciphering movements of non-rigid objects. , 2010, Journal of vision.
[42] G. Orban,et al. Responses of macaque STS neurons to optic flow components: a comparison of areas MT and MST. , 1994, Journal of neurophysiology.
[43] G. Mather,et al. Gender discrimination in biological motion displays based on dynamic cues , 1994, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.
[44] Matthew Brand,et al. A direct method for 3D factorization of nonrigid motion observed in 2D , 2005, 2005 IEEE Computer Society Conference on Computer Vision and Pattern Recognition (CVPR'05).
[45] J J Koenderink,et al. Structure from motion: A tolerance analysis , 1996, Perception & psychophysics.
[46] Tim S. Meese,et al. On the relationship between deformation and perceived surface slant , 1996, Vision Research.
[47] J T Todd,et al. The perceptual analysis of structure from motion for rotating objects undergoing affine stretching transformations , 1993, Perception & psychophysics.
[48] Alexander Grunewald,et al. Neural Correlates of Structure-from-Motion Perception in Macaque V1 and MT , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[49] Takeo Kanade,et al. Nonrigid Structure from Motion in Trajectory Space , 2008, NIPS.
[50] Andrea J. van Doorn,et al. Invariant Properties of the Motion Parallax Field due to the Movement of Rigid Bodies Relative to an Observer , 1975 .
[51] T. Meese,et al. Computation of surface slant from optic flow: Orthogonal components of speed gradient can be combined , 1997, Vision Research.
[52] R. M. Siegel,et al. Three-dimensional structure-from-motion selectivity in the anterior superior temporal polysensory area, STPa, of the behaving monkey. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.
[53] B Rogers,et al. Motion Parallax as an Independent Cue for Depth Perception , 1979, Perception.
[54] C Caudek,et al. Distortions of depth-order relations and parallelism in structure from motion , 1998, Perception & psychophysics.
[55] R. Andersen,et al. Encoding of three-dimensional structure-from-motion by primate area MT neurons , 1998, Nature.
[56] S. Lea,et al. Perception of Emotion from Dynamic Point-Light Displays Represented in Dance , 1996, Perception.
[57] Guy Marchal,et al. Human Cortical Regions Involved in Extracting Depth from Motion , 1999, Neuron.
[58] Aaron Hertzmann,et al. Nonrigid Structure-from-Motion: Estimating Shape and Motion with Hierarchical Priors , 2008, IEEE Transactions on Pattern Analysis and Machine Intelligence.
[59] Takeo Kanade,et al. Shape and motion from image streams under orthography: a factorization method , 1992, International Journal of Computer Vision.
[60] G. Johansson. Visual perception of biological motion and a model for its analysis , 1973 .
[61] Ralph M. Siegel,et al. Optic Flow Selectivity in the Anterior Superior Temporal Polysensory Area, STPa, of the Behaving Monkey , 1999, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[62] Tomaso Poggio,et al. Role of learning in three-dimensional form perception , 1996, Nature.
[63] Donald D. Hoffman,et al. Discriminating rigid from nonrigid motion: Minimum points and views , 1990, Perception & psychophysics.
[64] Henning Biermann,et al. Recovering non-rigid 3D shape from image streams , 2000, Proceedings IEEE Conference on Computer Vision and Pattern Recognition. CVPR 2000 (Cat. No.PR00662).
[65] T. Albright,et al. Contribution of area MT to perception of three-dimensional shape: a computational study , 1996, Vision Research.
[66] D. Regan,et al. Shape discrimination and the judgement of perfect symmetry: Dissociation of shape from size , 1992, Vision Research.
[67] E C Hildreth,et al. Incremental rigidity scheme for recovering structure from motion: position-based versus velocity-based formulations. , 1987, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.
[68] J T Todd,et al. The perception of three-dimensional structure from rigid and nonrigid motion , 1984, Perception & psychophysics.