Voice and Emotion Processing in the Human Neonatal Brain

Although the voice-sensitive neural system emerges very early in development, it has yet to be demonstrated whether the neonatal brain is sensitive to voice perception. We measured the EEG mismatch response (MMR) elicited by emotionally spoken syllables “dada” along with correspondingly synthesized nonvocal sounds, whose fundamental frequency contours were matched, in 98 full-term newborns aged 1–5 days. In Experiment 1, happy syllables relative to nonvocal sounds elicited an MMR lateralized to the right hemisphere. In Experiment 2, fearful syllables elicited stronger amplitudes than happy or neutral syllables, and this response had no sex differences. In Experiment 3, angry versus happy syllables elicited an MMR, although their corresponding nonvocal sounds did not. Here, we show that affective discrimination is selectively driven by voice processing per se rather than low-level acoustical features and that the cerebral specialization for human voice and emotion processing emerges over the right hemisphere during the first days of life.

[1]  P. Belin,et al.  An electrophysiological correlate of voice processing in 4- to 5-year-old children. , 2010, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[2]  M. Huotilainen A new dimension on foetal language learning , 2013, Acta paediatrica.

[3]  Anne-Lise Giraud,et al.  Distinct functional substrates along the right superior temporal sulcus for the processing of voices , 2004, NeuroImage.

[4]  C. Hutt,et al.  Habituation in relation to State in the Human Neonate , 1968, Nature.

[5]  K. Scherer,et al.  Acoustic profiles in vocal emotion expression. , 1996, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[6]  Angela D. Friederici,et al.  When vocal processing gets emotional: On the role of social orientation in relevance detection by the human amygdala , 2008, NeuroImage.

[7]  Mark H. Johnson,et al.  The emergence of cerebral specialization for the human voice over the first months of life , 2012, Social neuroscience.

[8]  H. Gomes,et al.  Mismatch Negativity in Children with Autism and Typical Development , 2008, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[9]  D. Grandjean,et al.  Processing of emotional vocalizations in bilateral inferior frontal cortex , 2013, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[10]  Pascal Belin,et al.  Mother and stranger: an electrophysiological study of voice processing in newborns. , 2011, Cerebral cortex.

[11]  A. Walker-Andrews Infants' perception of expressive behaviors: differentiation of multimodal information. , 1997, Psychological bulletin.

[12]  N. Logothetis,et al.  Where Are the Human Speech and Voice Regions, and Do Other Animals Have Anything Like Them? , 2009, The Neuroscientist : a review journal bringing neurobiology, neurology and psychiatry.

[13]  Angela D. Friederici,et al.  The Developmental Origins of Voice Processing in the Human Brain , 2010, Neuron.

[14]  M. Erb,et al.  Effects of prosodic emotional intensity on activation of associative auditory cortex , 2006, Neuroreport.

[15]  T. Grossmann The development of emotion perception in face and voice during infancy. , 2010, Restorative neurology and neuroscience.

[16]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Functional Neuroimaging of Speech Perception in Infants , 2002, Science.

[17]  P. Jusczyk,et al.  A precursor of language acquisition in young infants , 1988, Cognition.

[18]  D. Brandeis,et al.  Development of the automatic mismatch response: from frontal positivity in kindergarten children to the mismatch negativity , 2003, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[19]  L. Bahrick,et al.  The development of infant discrimination of affect in multimodal and unimodal stimulation: The role of intersensory redundancy. , 2007, Developmental psychology.

[20]  Mark H. Johnson,et al.  Infancy and autism: progress, prospects, and challenges. , 2007, Progress in brain research.

[21]  A. Friederici,et al.  Neural manifestation of cognitive and precognitive mismatch detection in early infancy , 2002, Neuroreport.

[22]  M. Vince,et al.  Responses of neonates to parents' and others' voices. , 1988, Early human development.

[23]  C. Zahn-Waxler,et al.  Concern for Others in the First Year of Life: Theory, Evidence, and Avenues for Research , 2013 .

[24]  J. Decety,et al.  The Role of Affect in the Neurodevelopment of Morality , 2013 .

[25]  Bartolo Lanuzza,et al.  The mismatch negativity and the P3a components of the auditory event-related potentials in autistic low-functioning subjects , 2003, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[26]  G Turkewitz,et al.  Asymmetric headturning to speech and nonspeech in human newborns. , 1996, Developmental psychobiology.

[27]  K. Scherer,et al.  The voices of wrath: brain responses to angry prosody in meaningless speech , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.

[28]  S. Baron-Cohen,et al.  Reading the Mind in the Voice: A Study with Normal Adults and Adults with Asperger Syndrome and High Functioning Autism , 2002, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[29]  R. Näätänen,et al.  The mismatch negativity (MMN) in basic research of central auditory processing: A review , 2007, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[30]  Y. Konishi,et al.  The relationship between mismatch negativity and arousal level. Can mismatch negativity be an index for evaluating the arousal level in infants? , 2002, Sleep medicine.

[31]  R. K. Simpson Nature Neuroscience , 2022 .

[32]  Paul Boersma,et al.  Praat, a system for doing phonetics by computer , 2002 .

[33]  Mark H. Johnson,et al.  Early Specialization for Voice and Emotion Processing in the Infant Brain , 2011, Current Biology.

[34]  Angela D. Friederici,et al.  Sex differences in the preattentive processing of vocal emotional expressions , 2005, Neuroreport.

[35]  A. Friederici,et al.  When during development do our brains get tuned to the human voice? , 2012, Social neuroscience.

[36]  Jean Decety,et al.  The Neurodevelopment of Empathy in Humans , 2010, Developmental Neuroscience.

[37]  J. Ahveninen,et al.  Atypical mismatch negativity to distressful voices associated with conduct disorder symptoms. , 2013, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[38]  A. Decasper,et al.  Of Human Bonding: Newborns Prefer Their Mothers' Voices , 1980 .

[39]  R. D. Clark,et al.  Distress crying in neonates: Species and peer specificity. , 1982 .

[40]  Giacomo Vivanti,et al.  Are emotion impairments unique to, universal, or specific in autism spectrum disorder? A comprehensive review , 2013, Cognition & emotion.

[41]  K. Reinikainen,et al.  Mismatch negativity indicates vowel discrimination in newborns , 1995, Hearing Research.

[42]  R. Näätänen,et al.  Electric Brain Responses Obtained From Newborn Infants to Changes in Duration in Complex Harmonic Tones , 2002, Developmental neuropsychology.

[43]  P. Belin,et al.  Thinking the voice: neural correlates of voice perception , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[44]  G. Csibra,et al.  Electrophysiological Methods in Studying Infant Cognitive Development , 2009 .

[45]  N. Logothetis,et al.  A voice region in the monkey brain , 2008, Nature Neuroscience.

[46]  Pascal Belin,et al.  Human voice perception , 2011, Current Biology.

[47]  S. Hains,et al.  Effects of Experience on Fetal Voice Recognition , 2003, Psychological science.

[48]  R. Hobson,et al.  Naming emotion in faces and voices: Abilities and disabilities in autism and mental retardation , 1989 .

[49]  N. Fox,et al.  If it's not left, it's right. Electroencephalograph asymmetry and the development of emotion. , 1991, The American psychologist.

[50]  R. Näätänen,et al.  Event-related potential features indexing central auditory discrimination by newborns. , 2002, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[51]  P. Leppänen,et al.  Mismatch negativity (MMN) as a tool for investigating auditory discrimination and sensory memory in infants and children , 2000, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[52]  William J. Talkington,et al.  Using naturalistic utterances to investigate vocal communication processing and development in human and non-human primates , 2013, Hearing Research.

[53]  P. Belin,et al.  Before Speech: Cerebral Voice Processing in Infants , 2010, Neuron.

[54]  Jean Decety,et al.  Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences the Neuroevolution of Empathy , 2022 .

[55]  P. Vuilleumier,et al.  How brains beware: neural mechanisms of emotional attention , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[56]  William P. Fifer,et al.  Two-day-olds prefer their native language , 1993 .

[57]  Pascal Belin,et al.  Electrophysiological evidence for an early processing of human voices , 2009, BMC Neuroscience.

[58]  F. Simion,et al.  Can newborns discriminate between their own cry and the cry of another newborn infant? , 1999, Developmental psychology.

[59]  Angela D. Friederici,et al.  Localization of the syntactic mismatch negativity in the temporal cortex: An MEG study , 2009, NeuroImage.

[60]  R. Zatorre,et al.  Voice-selective areas in human auditory cortex , 2000, Nature.

[61]  Prenatal experience and neonatal responsiveness to vocal expressions of emotion. , 1999, Developmental psychobiology.

[62]  D. Lancker,et al.  Recognition of emotional‐prosodic meanings in speech by autistic, schizophrenic, and normal children , 1989 .

[63]  P. Huttenlocher,et al.  Regional differences in synaptogenesis in human cerebral cortex , 1997, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[64]  T. Striano,et al.  Infants' electric brain responses to emotional prosody , 2005, Neuroreport.

[65]  A. Friederici Neurophysiological markers of early language acquisition: from syllables to sentences , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[66]  Pascal Belin,et al.  Abnormal cortical voice processing in autism , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.