Object-based selection operates on a grouped array of locations

In two experiments, space- and object-based selection effects were investigated, using variants of the ring-cuing paradigm of Egly and Homa (1984). The results revealed significant cuing modulation for nonring configurations of target locations spanning a range of retinal eccentricities, with the cuing effects independent of eccentricity and confined to the configuration of locations (rather than extending to locations within the space enclosed by the cued configuration). These results are consistent with object-based selection operating on a grouped spatial array (Vecera & Farah, 1994). Object selection may be based on a supradimensional saliency map representation of the field, modulated by feature-specific segmentation mechanisms (e.g., an object may be made salient on the basis of its color). Complex objects may be represented by grouped saliency signals. In this way, a two-dimensional spatial (saliency) representation may provide the common format for object-based selection, prior to full object definition.

[1]  H. Müller,et al.  Dimension-based visual attention modulates dual-judgment accuracy in Duncan's (1984) one- versus two-object report paradigm. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[2]  Stephen Handel,et al.  Effect of Element Composition on Streaming and the Ordering of Repeating Sequences. , 1977 .

[3]  J. Hoffman,et al.  Spatial selectivity in visual search , 1981, Perception & psychophysics.

[4]  M. Posner,et al.  Attention and the detection of signals. , 1980, Journal of experimental psychology.

[5]  D L Fisher,et al.  Limited-channel models of automatic detection: capacity and scanning in visual search. , 1982, Psychological review.

[6]  G. Humphreys,et al.  Selection by color and form in vision. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[7]  Charles Curtis Eriksen,et al.  The extent of processing of noise elements during selective encoding from visual displays , 1973 .

[8]  I. Biederman Recognition-by-components: a theory of human image understanding. , 1987, Psychological review.

[9]  Donald L. Fisher,et al.  Central capacity limits in consistent mapping, visual search tasks: Four channels or more? , 1984, Cognitive Psychology.

[10]  H J Müller,et al.  Visual search for singleton feature targets within and across feature dimensions , 1995, Perception & psychophysics.

[11]  M. Cheal,et al.  Benefits from attention depend on the target type in location-precued discrimination. , 1992, Acta psychologica.

[12]  P Jolicoeur,et al.  Orientation congruency effects in visual search. , 1992, Canadian journal of psychology.

[13]  J. Jonides,et al.  On the Cost and Benefit of Cost and Benefit , 1984 .

[14]  H. Pashler,et al.  Close binding of identity and location in visual feature perception. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[15]  G. Baylis,et al.  Visual attention and objects: evidence for hierarchical coding of location. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[16]  S. Yantis Stimulus-driven attentional capture and attentional control settings. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[17]  S. Yantis,et al.  Mechanisms of attentional priority. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[18]  M. Posner,et al.  Orienting of Attention* , 1980, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[19]  J. Wolfe,et al.  Guided Search 2.0 A revised model of visual search , 1994, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[20]  S. Yantis Multielement visual tracking: Attention and perceptual organization , 1992, Cognitive Psychology.

[21]  J. Duncan,et al.  Visual search and stimulus similarity. , 1989, Psychological review.

[22]  M. Farah,et al.  Does visual attention select objects or locations? , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[23]  D. Dorfman,et al.  Maximum-likelihood estimation of parameters of signal-detection theory and determination of confidence intervals—Rating-method data , 1969 .

[24]  David Marr,et al.  VISION A Computational Investigation into the Human Representation and Processing of Visual Information , 2009 .

[25]  A. Treisman,et al.  Conjunction search revisited , 1990 .

[26]  R W Remington,et al.  The structure of attentional control: contingent attentional capture by apparent motion, abrupt onset, and color. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[27]  W. Schneider,et al.  Space-based visual attention models and object selection: Constraints, problems, and possible solutions , 1993, Psychological research.

[28]  D. Homa,et al.  Sensitization of the visual field. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[29]  Jon Driver,et al.  One-Sided Edge Assignment in Vision: 2. Part Decomposition, Shape Description, and Attention to Objects , 1995 .

[30]  S. Palmer Common region: A new principle of perceptual grouping , 1992, Cognitive Psychology.

[31]  S. Palmer,et al.  Rethinking perceptual organization: The role of uniform connectedness , 1994, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[32]  J. Duncan Cooperating brain systems in selective perception and action. , 1996 .

[33]  A. Treisman,et al.  A feature-integration theory of attention , 1980, Cognitive Psychology.

[34]  B. Gibson Visual attention and objects: one versus two or convex versus concave? , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[35]  Jon Driver,et al.  One-Sided Edge Assignment in Vision: 1. Figure-Ground Segmentation and Attention to Objects , 1995 .

[36]  J. Juola,et al.  Voluntary control of attention near the fovea. , 1987, Acta psychologica.

[37]  A. H. C. van der Heijden,et al.  The role of position in object selection in vision , 1993 .

[38]  P. Jolicoeur,et al.  Rotated object identification with and without orientation cues. , 1995, Canadian journal of experimental psychology = Revue canadienne de psychologie experimentale.

[39]  N. Lavie Perceptual load as a necessary condition for selective attention. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[40]  Donald D. Hoffman,et al.  Parts of recognition , 1984, Cognition.

[41]  R. Rafal,et al.  Shifting visual attention between objects and locations: evidence from normal and parietal lesion subjects. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[42]  G W Humphreys,et al.  Luminance-increment detection: capacity-limited or not? , 1991, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[43]  C. J. Downing Expectancy and visual-spatial attention: effects on perceptual quality. , 1988, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[44]  A F Kramer,et al.  Object-based attentional selection--grouped arrays or spatially invariant representations?: comment on vecera and Farah (1994). , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[45]  S Ullman,et al.  Shifts in selective visual attention: towards the underlying neural circuitry. , 1985, Human neurobiology.

[46]  J. Duncan Selective attention and the organization of visual information , 1984 .