Aversive learning shapes neuronal orientation tuning in human visual cortex

The responses of sensory cortical neurons are shaped by experience. As a result perceptual biases evolve, selectively facilitating the detection and identification of sensory events that are relevant for adaptive behaviour. Here we examine the involvement of human visual cortex in the formation of learned perceptual biases. We use classical aversive conditioning to associate one out of a series of oriented gratings with a noxious sound stimulus. After as few as two grating-sound pairings, visual cortical responses to the sound-paired grating show selective amplification. Furthermore, as learning progresses, responses to the orientations with greatest similarity to the sound-paired grating are increasingly suppressed, suggesting inhibitory interactions between orientation-selective neuronal populations. Changes in cortical connectivity between occipital and fronto-temporal regions mirror the changes in visuo-cortical response amplitudes. These findings suggest that short-term behaviourally driven retuning of human visual cortical neurons involves distal top–down projections as well as local inhibitory interactions.

[1]  Janneke F. M. Jehee,et al.  Less Is More: Expectation Sharpens Representations in the Primary Visual Cortex , 2012, Neuron.

[2]  Andreas Keil,et al.  Adaptation in human visual cortex as a mechanism for rapid discrimination of aversive stimuli , 2007, NeuroImage.

[3]  Diego A. Gutnisky,et al.  Attention Alters Visual Plasticity during Exposure-Based Learning , 2009, Current Biology.

[4]  Dario L. Ringach,et al.  Link between orientation and retinotopic maps in primary visual cortex , 2012, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[5]  Donald G. Langsley Technology in Mental Health Care Delivery Systems , 1982 .

[6]  A. Kohn,et al.  Distinct Effects of Brief and Prolonged Adaptation on Orientation Tuning in Primary Visual Cortex , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[7]  Christian Grillon,et al.  Neural substrates of classically conditioned fear-generalization in humans: a parametric fMRI study. , 2014, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[8]  S. Lissek TOWARD AN ACCOUNT OF CLINICAL ANXIETY PREDICATED ON BASIC, NEURALLY MAPPED MECHANISMS OF PAVLOVIAN FEAR‐LEARNING: THE CASE FOR CONDITIONED OVERGENERALIZATION , 2012, Depression and anxiety.

[9]  A. Keil,et al.  Visuocortical changes during delay and trace aversive conditioning: evidence from steady-state visual evoked potentials. , 2013, Emotion.

[10]  R. Shapley,et al.  Dynamics of Orientation Selectivity in the Primary Visual Cortex and the Importance of Cortical Inhibition , 2003, Neuron.

[11]  R. Freeman,et al.  Origins of cross-orientation suppression in the visual cortex. , 2006, Journal of neurophysiology.

[12]  W. K. Honig,et al.  Stimulus control after extradimensional training in pigeons: A comparison of response contingent and noncontingent training procedures. , 1974 .

[13]  C. Spielberger,et al.  Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory , 1970 .

[14]  Andreas Keil,et al.  Perceiving Threat In the Face of Safety: Excitation and Inhibition of Conditioned Fear in Human Visual Cortex , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[15]  S. Hochstein,et al.  The reverse hierarchy theory of visual perceptual learning , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[16]  A. Keil,et al.  Differential classical conditioning selectively heightens response gain of neural population activity in human visual cortex. , 2014, Psychophysiology.

[17]  B. Rockstroh,et al.  Mapping EEG-potentials on the surface of the brain: A strategy for uncovering cortical sources , 2005, Brain Topography.

[18]  Andreas Keil,et al.  Fear but not awareness predicts enhanced sensory processing in fear conditioning. , 2006, Psychophysiology.

[19]  Yang Dan,et al.  Dynamic Modification of Cortical Orientation Tuning Mediated by Recurrent Connections , 2002, Neuron.

[20]  Markus Junghöfer,et al.  ElectroMagnetoEncephalography Software: Overview and Integration with Other EEG/MEG Toolboxes , 2011, Comput. Intell. Neurosci..

[21]  M. Bradley,et al.  Orienting and Emotional Perception: Facilitation, Attenuation, and Interference , 2012, Front. Psychology.

[22]  A. Keil,et al.  Temporal Trade-Off Effects in Sustained Attention: Dynamics in Visual Cortex Predict the Target Detection Performance during Distraction , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[23]  M. Carandini,et al.  Suppression without Inhibition in Visual Cortex , 2002, Neuron.

[24]  Terrence J. Sejnowski,et al.  The effect of neural adaptation on population coding accuracy , 2011, Journal of Computational Neuroscience.

[25]  D H Brainard,et al.  The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[26]  V. Arolt,et al.  Human Fear Conditioning and Extinction in Neuroimaging: A Systematic Review , 2009, PloS one.

[27]  A. Ohman,et al.  Emotion drives attention: detecting the snake in the grass. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[28]  N. Guttman,et al.  Discriminability and stimulus generalization. , 1956, Journal of experimental psychology.

[29]  N. Weinberger Specific long-term memory traces in primary auditory cortex , 2004, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[30]  Marisa Carrasco,et al.  When sustained attention impairs perception , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.

[31]  D. Ringach Population coding under normalization , 2010, Vision Research.

[32]  S A Hillyard,et al.  Feature-selective attention enhances color signals in early visual areas of the human brain , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[33]  Drew B Headley,et al.  Gamma-Band Activation Predicts Both Associative Memory and Cortical Plasticity , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[34]  M. Bradley,et al.  Electrocortical and electrodermal responses covary as a function of emotional arousal: a single-trial analysis. , 2008, Psychophysiology.

[35]  E. Maris Statistical testing in electrophysiological studies. , 2012, Psychophysiology.

[36]  D. Spinelli,et al.  Spatiotemporal analysis of the cortical sources of the steady‐state visual evoked potential , 2007, Human brain mapping.

[37]  E. Bernat,et al.  Event-related brain correlates of associative learning without awareness. , 2004, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[38]  M. Sur,et al.  Adaptation-Induced Plasticity of Orientation Tuning in Adult Visual Cortex , 2000, Neuron.

[39]  A. Keil,et al.  Modulation of the C1 visual event-related component by conditioned stimuli: evidence for sensory plasticity in early affective perception. , 2006, Cerebral cortex.

[40]  B Kissin,et al.  Evoked cortical responses to affective visual stimuli. , 1967, Psychophysiology.

[41]  Michael J. Gray,et al.  Luminance, but not chromatic visual pathways, mediate amplification of conditioned danger signals in human visual cortex , 2013, The European journal of neuroscience.

[42]  Andreas Keil,et al.  Acquired fears reflected in cortical sensory processing: a review of electrophysiological studies of human classical conditioning. , 2012, Psychophysiology.

[43]  M. Fahle Perceptual learning: specificity versus generalization , 2005, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[44]  W. Freeman The physiology of perception. , 1991, Scientific American.

[45]  F. Varela,et al.  Measuring phase synchrony in brain signals , 1999, Human brain mapping.

[46]  Andreas Keil,et al.  Selective Processing of Multiple Features in the Human Brain: Effects of Feature Type and Salience , 2011, PloS one.

[47]  R. Shapley,et al.  Dynamics of orientation tuning in macaque V1: the role of global and tuned suppression. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.