Cognition and Emotion

Research on self-other framing suggests that self-judgements typically neglect information about others and instead use an ambiguous intrapersonal standard, making these judgements susceptible to the influence of mood as predicted by the affect infusion model (Forgas, 1995). Two experiments tested the hypothesis that mood-congruent judgements of personal success occur when these judgements are framed to maintain self-focus but are eliminated when participants first focus on other individuals. In Experiment 1, self-other framing moderated the influence of mood on perceived success for an ambiguous object identification task. Happy participants reported greater perceived success, compared to sad participants, after judging their own success relative to others (self-focused frame) but not after directly judging the success of others (other-focused frame). Experiment 2 replicated these results with students' perceptions of their academic success. These findings suggest that the open, constructive processing accompanying most self-judgements is critical in producing mood-congruent perceptions of personal success.

[1]  L. Festinger A Theory of Social Comparison Processes , 1954 .

[2]  E. Velten A laboratory task for induction of mood states. , 1968, Behaviour research and therapy.

[3]  A. Isen,et al.  Effect of feeling good on helping: cookies and kindness. , 1972, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[4]  M. Ross,et al.  Egocentric Biases in Availability and Attribution , 1979 .

[5]  Focus of attention mediates the impact of negative affect on altruism. , 1980 .

[6]  N. Weinstein Unrealistic optimism about future life events , 1980 .

[7]  P. Salovey,et al.  The joys of helping: Focus of attention mediates the impact of positive affect on altruism. , 1981 .

[8]  T. B. Rogers A model of the self as an aspect of the human information processing system , 1981 .

[9]  G. Bower Mood and memory. , 1981, The American psychologist.

[10]  J. Williams,et al.  Mood-induction research--its implications for clinical depression. , 1982, Behaviour research and therapy.

[11]  A. Bandura Self-efficacy mechanism in human agency , 2024, Psihologìâ ì suspìlʹstvo.

[12]  Walter Mischel,et al.  Influence of affect on cognitive social learning person variables. , 1982 .

[13]  G. Clore,et al.  Mood, misattribution, and judgments of well-being: Informative and directive functions of affective states. , 1983 .

[14]  A. Parducci Value Judgments: Toward a Relational Theory of Happiness , 1984 .

[15]  A. Bandura Social Foundations of Thought and Action: A Social Cognitive Theory , 1985 .

[16]  P. H. Blaney Affect and memory: a review. , 1986, Psychological bulletin.

[17]  Shelley E. Taylor,et al.  Affect and the processing of personal information: Evidence for mood-activated self-schemata☆ , 1986 .

[18]  J. Suls Notes on the Occasion of Social Comparison Theory's Thirtieth Birthday , 1986 .

[19]  G. Bower,et al.  Mood effects on person-perception judgments. , 1987, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[20]  F. Strack,et al.  Inhibiting and facilitating conditions of the human smile: a nonobtrusive test of the facial feedback hypothesis. , 1988, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[21]  S. E. Taylor,et al.  Social comparison activity under threat: downward evaluation and upward contacts. , 1989, Psychological review.

[22]  Z. Kunda,et al.  The case for motivated reasoning. , 1990, Psychological bulletin.

[23]  Does affect induce self-focused attention? , 1990, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[24]  J. Karylowski Social reference points and accessibility of trait-related information in self-other similarity judgments , 1990 .

[25]  J. Suls,et al.  Social comparison: Contemporary theory and research. , 1991 .

[26]  L. Wheeler A brief history of social comparison theory. , 1991 .

[27]  Edward McAuley,et al.  Self-Efficacy, Perceptions of Success, and Intrinsic Motivation for Exercise1 , 1991 .

[28]  John D. Mayer,et al.  Mood-congruent judgment is a general effect. , 1992 .

[29]  P Salovey Mood-induced self-focused attention. , 1992, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[30]  J. D. Brown,et al.  Self-esteem, mood, and self-evaluation: changes in mood and the way you see you. , 1993, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[31]  Z. Kunda,et al.  Maintaining Self-Serving Social Comparisons: Biased Reconstruction of One's Past Behaviors , 1993 .

[32]  R. Petty,et al.  Positive Mood and Persuasion: Different Roles for Affect Under High- and Low-Elaboration Conditions , 1993 .

[33]  D. Cervone,et al.  Mood, Self-Efficacy, and Performance Standards: Lower Moods Induce Higher Standards for Performance , 1994 .

[34]  R. Petty,et al.  Personality moderators of mood congruency effects on cognition: the role of self-esteem and negative mood regulation. , 1995, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[35]  C. Sedikides,et al.  Central and peripheral self-conceptions are differentially influenced by mood: tests of the differential sensitivity hypothesis. , 1995, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[36]  M. L. Klotz,et al.  Personal contact, individuation, and the better-than-average effect. , 1995 .

[37]  J. Forgas Mood and judgment: the affect infusion model (AIM). , 1995, Psychological bulletin.

[38]  C. L. Rusting,et al.  Personality, mood, and cognitive processing of emotional information: three conceptual frameworks. , 1998, Psychological bulletin.

[39]  Joseph P. Forgas,et al.  On feeling good and being rude: Affective influences on language use and request formulations. , 1999 .

[40]  J. Kruger Lake Wobegon be gone! The "below-average effect" and the egocentric nature of comparative ability judgments. , 1999, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[41]  J. Eccles,et al.  Expectancy-Value Theory of Achievement Motivation. , 2000, Contemporary educational psychology.

[42]  L. F. Barrett,et al.  Handbook of emotions, 2nd ed. , 2000 .

[43]  Gordon H. Bower,et al.  Mood and Social Memory , 2000 .

[44]  J. Eiser,et al.  Optimism, Pessimism, and the Direction of Self–Other Comparisons , 2001 .

[45]  G. Gendolla,et al.  The informational impact of mood on effort mobilization: a study of cardiovascular and electrodermal responses. , 2001, Emotion.

[46]  Jeffrey D. Green,et al.  Affective influences on the self-concept: qualifying the mood congruency principle , 2001 .

[47]  D. Hirshleifer,et al.  Good Day Sunshine: Stock Returns and the Weather , 2001 .

[48]  J. Forgas Handbook of Affect and Social Cognition , 2001 .

[49]  M. Alicke,et al.  The “Better Than Myself Effect” , 2001 .

[50]  E. Higgins,et al.  PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Article ENJOYING GOAL-DIRECTED ACTION: The Role of Regulatory Fit , 2022 .

[51]  J. Forgas Feeling and Doing: Affective Influences on Interpersonal Behavior , 2002 .

[52]  Joseph P. Forgas,et al.  Toward Understanding the Role of Affect in Social Thinking and Behavior , 2002 .

[53]  John R. Chambers,et al.  Biases in social comparative judgments: the role of nonmotivated factors in above-average and comparative-optimism effects. , 2004, Psychological bulletin.

[54]  J. Eiser,et al.  The focus effect and self-positivity in ratings of self-other similarity and difference. , 2006, The British journal of social psychology.