Avoidance or Vigilance? The Psychology of False-Positive Test Results

The influence of false-positive results on consumers' decisions to get retested in high-stakes domains is examined. Findings across four laboratory experiments indicate that a false-positive outcome increases perceptions of vulnerability and test inaccuracy, even holding constant test-error base rates. Increased perceived vulnerability appears to be directly related to the testing event, as the effects are not replicated by simply asking subjects to imagine having the malady. The findings also show that a false-positive result increases planned compliance if there are poor alternatives to testing or if the value of test-initiated treatment is high but does not affect compliance if good testing alternatives are available or the treatment value is low. Using a pooled analysis across multiple studies, the results of a false-positive outcome on compliance are shown to be partially mediated by changes in perceived vulnerability and test inaccuracy. Public-policy implications regarding individual decision behavior and professional test administration are discussed. Copyright 1999 by the University of Chicago.

[1]  E. Pisano,et al.  Screening mammography behavior after a false positive mammogram. , 1998, Cancer detection and prevention.

[2]  I. Rosenstock Why people use health services. , 1966, The Milbank Memorial Fund quarterly.

[3]  D. Lehmann Market research and analysis , 1979 .

[4]  I. Gram,et al.  Cancer anxiety and attitudes toward mammography among screening attenders, nonattenders, and women never invited. , 1992, American journal of public health.

[5]  J. Wardle,et al.  The psychological costs of screening for cancer. , 1992, Journal of psychosomatic research.

[6]  M. Bar-Hillel The base-rate fallacy in probability judgments. , 1980 .

[7]  Lauren G. Block,et al.  When to Accentuate the Negative: The Effects of Perceived Efficacy and Message Framing on Intentions to Perform a Health-Related Behavior , 1995 .

[8]  JohannesG. Schmidt,et al.  The epidemiology of mass breast cancer screening--a plea for a valid measure of benefit. , 1990, Journal of clinical epidemiology.

[9]  Punam Anand,et al.  The Effect of Vivid Attributes on the Evaluation of Alternatives: The Role of Differential Attention and Cognitive Elaboration , 1989 .

[10]  Lauren G. Block,et al.  Effects of Self-Efficacy and Vividness on the Persuasiveness of Health Communications , 1997 .

[11]  Yaacov Trope,et al.  Problem solving in judgment under uncertainty. , 1987 .

[12]  Icek Ajzen,et al.  From Intentions to Actions: A Theory of Planned Behavior , 1985 .

[13]  R W Rogers,et al.  Fear appeals and attitude change: effects of a threat's noxiousness, probability of occurrence, and the efficacy of coping responses. , 1976, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[14]  Chezy Ofir,et al.  Context Effects on Judgment under Uncertainty , 1984 .

[15]  H. Leventhal,et al.  Findings and Theory in the Study of Fear Communications , 1970 .

[16]  I. Ajzen,et al.  Understanding Attitudes and Predicting Social Behavior , 1980 .

[17]  L. Ross,et al.  Perseverance of Social Theories: The Role of Explanation in the Persistence of Discredited Information , 1980 .

[18]  J. Baron,et al.  Heuristics and biases in diagnostic reasoning: I. Priors, error costs, and test accuracy , 1988 .

[19]  Lauren G. Block,et al.  Vividness Effects: A Resource-Matching Perspective , 1997 .

[20]  Eugene Borgida,et al.  Attribution and the psychology of prediction. , 1975 .

[21]  L. Ross,et al.  Persistence of inaccurate beliefs about the self: perseverance effects in the classroom. , 1986, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[22]  R. W. Rogers,et al.  Effects of components of protection-motivation theory on adaptive and maladaptive coping with a health threat. , 1987, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[23]  B. Trock,et al.  Psychological and behavioral implications of abnormal mammograms. , 1991, Annals of internal medicine.

[24]  B. J. Winer Statistical Principles in Experimental Design , 1992 .

[25]  R. W. Rogers,et al.  A Protection Motivation Theory of Fear Appeals and Attitude Change1. , 1975, The Journal of psychology.

[26]  J. Bloom,et al.  Hypertension labeling and sense of well-being. , 1981, American journal of public health.

[27]  G. Bodegård,et al.  Four‐Year Follow‐up of Psychological Reactions to False Positive Screening Tests for Congenital Hypothyroidism , 1987, Acta paediatrica Scandinavica.

[28]  I. Rosenstock Historical Origins of the Health Belief Model , 1974 .

[29]  J. Swets,et al.  A decision-making theory of visual detection. , 1954, Psychological review.

[30]  R. Cialdini,et al.  Imagining Can Heighten or Lower the Perceived Likelihood of Contracting a Disease , 1985 .

[31]  J. Swets Indices of discrimination or diagnostic accuracy: their ROCs and implied models. , 1986, Psychological bulletin.

[32]  Howard Raiffa,et al.  Decision analysis: introductory lectures on choices under uncertainty. 1968. , 1969, M.D.Computing.

[33]  J. Sorenson,et al.  Parental response to repeat testing of infants with 'false-positive' results in a newborn screening program. , 1984, Pediatrics.

[34]  R. Andersen A behavioral model of families' use of health services , 1968 .

[35]  J. Elmore,et al.  Ten-year risk of false positive screening mammograms and clinical breast examinations. , 1998, The New England journal of medicine.

[36]  Dale T. Miller,et al.  Norm theory: Comparing reality to its alternatives , 1986 .