Background. Clinicians and researchers often wish to know how patients perceive the likelihoods of health risks. Little work has been done to develop and validate scales and formats to measure perceptions of event probabilities, particularly low probabilities (i.e., <1%). Objective. To compare a new visual analog scale with three benchmarks in terms of validity and reliability. Design. Survey with retest after approximately two weeks. Respondents estimated the probabilities of six events with the new scale, which featured a "magnifying glass" to represent probabilities between 0 and 1% on a logarithmic scale. Participants estimated the same probabilities on three benchmarks: two linear visual analog scales (one labeled with words, one with numbers) and a "1 in x" scale. Subjects. 100 veterans and family members and 107 university faculty and students. Measures. For each scale, the authors assessed: 1) validity—the correlation between participants' direct rankings (i.e., numbering them from 1 to 6) and scale-derived rankings of the relative probabilities of six events; 2) test-retest reliability— the correlation of responses from test to retest two weeks later; 3) usability (missing/ incorrect responses, participant evaluation). Results. Both the magnifier and the two linear scales outperformed the "1 in x" scale on all criteria. The magnifier scale performed about as well as the two linear visual analog scales for validity (correlation between direct and scale-derived rankings = 0.72), reliability (test-retest correlation = 0.55), and usability (2% missing or incorrect responses, 65% rated it easy to use). 62% felt the magnifier scale was a "very good or good" indicator of their feelings about chance. The magnifier scale facilitated expression of low-probability judgments. For example, the estimated chance of parenting sextuplets was orders of magnitude lower on the magnifier scale (median perceived chance 10 5) than on its linear counterpart (10-2). Participants' assessments of high-probability events (e.g., chance of catching a cold in the next year) were not affected by the presence of the magnifier. Conclusions. The "1 in x" scale performs poorly and is very difficult for people to use. The magnifier scale and the linear number scale are similar in validity, reliability, and usability. However, only the magnifier scale makes it possible to elicit perceptions in the low-probability range (<1%). Key words: patient perceptions; perception measurement scale. (Med Decis Making 2000;20:298-307)
[1]
G R Norman,et al.
Expressions of probability: words and numbers.
,
1980,
The New England journal of medicine.
[2]
J. Toogood,et al.
WHAT DO WE MEAN BY "USUALLY"?
,
1980,
The Lancet.
[3]
D. Grimes,et al.
Patients' understanding of medical risks: implications for genetic counseling.
,
1999,
Obstetrics and gynecology.
[4]
B. Fischhoff,et al.
Judged frequency of lethal events
,
1978
.
[5]
Amnon Rapoport,et al.
Measuring the Vague Meanings of Probability Terms
,
1986
.
[6]
B Fischhoff,et al.
Adolescent (in)vulnerability.
,
1993,
The American psychologist.
[7]
R F Nease,et al.
Perceptions of breast cancer risk and screening effectiveness in women younger than 50 years of age.
,
1995,
Journal of the National Cancer Institute.
[8]
Baruch Fischhoff,et al.
Fifty–fifty = 50%?
,
1999
.
[9]
Baruch Fischhoff,et al.
AIDS risk perceptions and decision biases.
,
1993
.
[10]
G. Glendon,et al.
Putting the risk of breast cancer in perspective.
,
1999,
The New England journal of medicine.
[11]
P. Slovic.
Perception of risk.
,
1987,
Science.
[12]
N D Weinstein,et al.
Scales for assessing perceptions of health hazard susceptibility.
,
1993,
Health education research.
[13]
Lisa M. Schwartz,et al.
The Role of Numeracy in Understanding the Benefit of Screening Mammography
,
1997,
Annals of Internal Medicine.
[14]
B. Fischhoff,et al.
How safe is safe enough? A psychometric study of attitudes towards technological risks and benefits
,
1978
.
[15]
M. Ruffin,et al.
Patients' interpretation of qualitative probability statements.
,
1994,
Archives of family medicine.
[16]
How medical professionals evaluate expressions of probability.
,
1987,
The New England journal of medicine.
[17]
D. Budescu,et al.
Processing Linguistic Probabilities: General Principles and Empirical Evidence
,
1995
.
[18]
Taylor Murray,et al.
Cancer statistics, 1998
,
1998,
CA: a cancer journal for clinicians.