Incidental formation of episodic associations: The importance of sentential context

The influence of relevant semantic context on the incidental formation of episodic associations between words was probed in two experiments. In Experiment 1, we examined the influence of associations formed incidentally between unrelated words presented either in isolation or embedded in a sentential context on subsequent explicit paired-associate learning tested by cued recall. The results of Experiment 1 showed that the cued-recall rate of words studied in sentential context was higher than that of words co-occurring in isolated pairs. A subsequent single-items recognition test showed equal item memory for words studied in sentences than for words studied in isolated pairs, suggesting that the sentential context effect in cued recall indeed reflected stronger associations between paired words rather than better memory for single words. In Experiment 2, we ruled out memory for the entire sentence as an alternative explanation for the results of Experiment 1. We suggest two possible mechanisms to account for this advantage: First, pairs embedded in a sentence undergo semantic elaboration that might lead to the incidental formation of an association between them. Second, words embedded in a sentence enjoy the conjoint activation of compatible semantic features, a fact that may also facilitate the formation of an episodic association between them. The implications of these results for computational models using word representations based on co-occurrence data are discussed. This research was supported by German-Israeli Science Foundation Grant 567 and by a doctoral research stipend to A. P. from the Israeli Foundation Trustees.

[1]  E. Tulving,et al.  Availability versus accessibility of information in memory for words , 1966 .

[2]  Acquisition of paired associates as a function of interpair interval , 1967 .

[3]  W. Epstein Recall of word lists following learning of sentences and of anomalous and random strings , 1969 .

[4]  S. I. Shapiro,et al.  Studies of Paired-Associate Learning as a Function of Normative Word-Association Strength , 1971 .

[5]  Mediated semantic compatibility in associative learning , 1972 .

[6]  F. Craik,et al.  Levels of Pro-cessing: A Framework for Memory Research , 1975 .

[7]  F. Berry,et al.  Stimulus selection in paired-associate learning: Consonant-triad versus word-triad paradigms , 1973 .

[8]  Fergus I. M. Craik,et al.  A “LEVELS OF ANALYSIS” VIEW OF MEMORY , 1973 .

[9]  F. Craik,et al.  Depth of processing and the retention of words , 1975 .

[10]  Morris Moscovitch,et al.  Depth of processing, retrieval cues, and uniqueness of encoding as factors in recall , 1976 .

[11]  F. Craik,et al.  Levels of Processing in Human Memory , 1979 .

[12]  D. Swinney Lexical access during sentence comprehension: (Re)consideration of context effects , 1979 .

[13]  D. Swinney,et al.  Accessing lexical ambiguities during sentence comprehension: Effects of frequency of meaning and contextual bias , 1981 .

[14]  G. Einstein,et al.  Relational and item-specific information in memory , 1981 .

[15]  L. Barsalou Context-independent and context-dependent information in concepts , 1982, Memory & cognition.

[16]  M. C. Smith,et al.  The relationship between contextual facilitation and depth of processing. , 1983, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[17]  J. Bransford,et al.  Elaboration and knowledge acquisition , 1984, Memory & cognition.

[18]  R. Ratcliff,et al.  Automatic activation of episodic information in a semantic memory task. , 1986, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[19]  D. Schacter,et al.  Effects of elaborative processing on implicit and explicit memory for new associations. , 1986 .

[20]  J. H. Neely,et al.  On obtaining episodic priming in a lexical decision task following paired-associate learning. , 1987, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[21]  P. Sherman The levels of analysis , 1988, Animal Behaviour.

[22]  Brian R. Flay,et al.  Levels of analysis , 1989 .

[23]  Daniel L. Schacter,et al.  Implicit memory: effects of elaboration depend on unitization , 1989 .

[24]  D Dagenbach,et al.  Adding new information to semantic memory: how much learning is enough to produce automatic priming? , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[25]  C. Burgess,et al.  Semantic and associative priming in the cerebral hemispheres: Some words do, some words don't … sometimes, some places , 1990, Brain and Language.

[26]  David A. Balota Comprehension Processes in Reading. , 1990 .

[27]  D. Spence,et al.  Lexical co-occurrence and association strength , 1990 .

[28]  G. Miller,et al.  Contextual correlates of semantic similarity , 1991 .

[29]  Bradford H. Challis,et al.  Level of processing affects priming in word fragment completion. , 1992 .

[30]  Randi C. Martin,et al.  How semantic is automatic semantic priming , 1992 .

[31]  W. Marslen-Wilson,et al.  Access to word meanings during spoken language comprehension: effects of sentential semantic context. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[32]  Roderick Hunt,et al.  The Enigma of Organization and Distinctiveness , 1993 .

[33]  Alan S. Brown,et al.  A reevaluation of semantic versus nonsemantic processing in implicit memory , 1994, Memory & cognition.

[34]  M. Moscovitch,et al.  Repetition priming for newly formed and preexisting associations: perceptual and conceptual influences. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[35]  R. Guttentag Children's associative learning: automatic and deliberate encoding of meaningful associations. , 1995, The American journal of psychology.

[36]  W. Marslen-Wilson,et al.  Accessing Different Types of Lexical Semantic Information: Evidence From Priming , 1995 .

[37]  M Moscovitch,et al.  Repetition priming effects for newly formed associations are perceptually based: evidence from shallow encoding and format specificity. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[38]  David C. Plaut,et al.  Semantic and Associative Priming in a Distributed Attractor Network , 1995 .

[39]  E. Reingold,et al.  Automatic Retrieval of New Associations under Shallow Encoding Conditions , 1996, Consciousness and Cognition.

[40]  Curt Burgess,et al.  Producing high-dimensional semantic spaces from lexical co-occurrence , 1996 .

[41]  T. Landauer,et al.  A Solution to Plato's Problem: The Latent Semantic Analysis Theory of Acquisition, Induction, and Representation of Knowledge. , 1997 .

[42]  J. Schrijnemakers,et al.  Adding new word associations to semantic memory: evidence for two interactive learning components. , 1997, Acta psychologica.

[43]  K. McRae,et al.  Automatic semantic similarity priming. , 1998 .

[44]  M Moscovitch,et al.  Levels of processing and selective attention effects on encoding in memory. , 1998, Acta psychologica.

[45]  W. Lowe,et al.  Modelling functional priming and the associative boost , 1998 .

[46]  Curt Burgess,et al.  From simple associations to the building blocks of language: Modeling meaning in memory with the HAL model , 1998 .

[47]  S. Dennis,et al.  What is free association and what does it measure? , 2000, Memory & cognition.

[48]  Yaron Silberman and Risto Miikkulainen and Shlomo Bentin Semantic Effect On Episodic Associations , 2001 .

[49]  N. P.,et al.  Meaning , amount of processing , and memory for words , 2022 .