Representational signalling in birds

Some animals give specific calls when they discover food or detect a particular type of predator. Companions respond with food-searching behaviour or by adopting appropriate escape responses. These signals thus seem to denote objects in the environment, but this specific mechanism has only been demonstrated for monkey alarm calls. We manipulated whether fowl (Gallus gallus) had recently found a small quantity of preferred food and then tested for a specific interaction between this event and their subsequent response to playback of food calls. In one treatment, food calls thus potentially provided information about the immediate environment, while in the other the putative message was redundant with individual experience. Food calls evoked substrate searching, but only if the hens had not recently discovered food. An identical manipulation had no effect on responses to an acoustically matched control call. These results show that chicken food calls are representational signals: they stimulate retrieval of information about a class of external events. This is the first such demonstration for any non-primate species. Representational signalling is hence more taxonomically widespread than has previously been thought, suggesting that it may be the product of common social factors, rather than an attribute of a particular phylogenetic lineage.

[1]  Christopher S. Evans,et al.  Chicken food calls are functionally referential , 1999, Animal Behaviour.

[2]  P. Holland Event representation in Pavlovian conditioning: Image and action , 1990, Cognition.

[3]  A. Stokes,et al.  THE FOOD CALL AND DISPLAY OF THE BOBWHITE QUAIL (COLINUS VIRGINIANUS) , 1968 .

[4]  R. Rescorla,et al.  Postconditioning devaluation of a reinforcer affects instrumental responding. , 1985 .

[5]  S. Gill,et al.  Functional reference in an alarm signal given during nest defence: seet calls of yellow warblers denote brood-parasitic brown-headed cowbirds , 2004, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[6]  D. Bickerton Language and Species , 1990 .

[7]  N. Collias,et al.  The Vocal Repertoire of the Red Junglefowl: A Spectrographic Classification and the Code of Communication , 1987 .

[8]  P. Marler,et al.  Food calling and audience effects in male chickens, Gallus gallus: their relationships to food availability, courtship and social facilitation , 1994, Animal Behaviour.

[9]  Christopher S. Evans,et al.  Referential Signals , 2000 .

[10]  A. W. Stokes,et al.  Courtship Feeding Calls in Gallinaceous Birds , 1972 .

[11]  M. D. Bitetti,et al.  Food-associated Calls of Tufted Capuchin Monkeys (Cebus Apella Nigritus) Are Functionally Referential Signals , 2003 .

[12]  P. Lieberman Human language and human uniqueness , 1994 .

[13]  Thomas Bugnyar,et al.  Food calling in ravens: are yells referential signals? , 2001, Animal Behaviour.

[14]  E. Morton,et al.  Animal Vocal Communication: A New Approach , 1998 .

[15]  Erick Greene,et al.  Allometry of Alarm Calls: Black-Capped Chickadees Encode Information About Predator Size , 2005, Science.

[16]  W. Fitch The Evolution of Language: A Comparative Review , 2005 .

[17]  M. Hauser The Evolution of Communication , 1996 .

[18]  K. Zuberbühler Referential labelling in Diana monkeys , 2000, Animal Behaviour.

[19]  P. Marler,et al.  Monkey responses to three different alarm calls: evidence of predator classification and semantic communication. , 1980, Science.

[20]  M. Manser,et al.  The acoustic structure of suricates' alarm calls varies with predator type and the level of response urgency , 2001, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[21]  W. Dittus Toque macaque food calls: Semantic communication concerning food distribution in the environment , 1984, Animal Behaviour.

[22]  K. Zuberbühler,et al.  Interspecies semantic communication in two forest primates , 2000, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[23]  C. Gallistel The organization of learning , 1990 .

[24]  C. Darwin The Descent of Man and Selection in Relation to Sex: INDEX , 1871 .

[25]  K. Zuberbühler Predator-specific alarm calls in Campbell's monkeys, Cercopithecus campbelli , 2001, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[26]  P. Tannenbaum,et al.  Food-associated calls correlate with food preferences in cotton-top tamarins , 1991, Animal Behaviour.

[27]  Klaus Zuberbühler,et al.  Conceptual Semantics in a Nonhuman Primate , 1999 .

[28]  Geoffrey Hall,et al.  Learning about associatively activated stimulus representations: Implications for acquired equivalence and perceptual learning , 1996 .

[29]  Klaus Zuberbühler,et al.  Functionally Referential Communication in a Chimpanzee , 2005, Current Biology.

[30]  M. Hauser,et al.  Functional referents and acoustic similarity: field playback experiments with rhesus monkeys , 1998, Animal Behaviour.

[31]  M. Manser,et al.  The information that receivers extract from alarm calls in suricates , 2001, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[32]  S. Pinker The Language Instinct , 1994 .