Emphasizing publishers does not effectively reduce susceptibility to misinformation on social media
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] K. Jamieson,et al. Processing the papal encyclical through perceptual filters: Pope Francis, identity-protective cognition, and climate change concern , 2017, Cognition.
[2] David G. Rand,et al. Who falls for fake news? The roles of bullshit receptivity, overclaiming, familiarity, and analytic thinking. , 2020, Journal of personality.
[3] David G. Rand,et al. Fighting misinformation on social media using crowdsourced judgments of news source quality , 2018, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[4] D. A. Kenny,et al. Treating stimuli as a random factor in social psychology: a new and comprehensive solution to a pervasive but largely ignored problem. , 2012, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[5] David G. Rand,et al. Belief in Fake News Is Associated with Delusionality, Dogmatism, Religious Fundamentalism, and Reduced Analytic Thinking , 2018, Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition.
[6] Alan R. Dennis,et al. Combating Fake News on Social Media with Source Ratings: The Effects of User and Expert Reputation Ratings , 2019, J. Manag. Inf. Syst..
[7] David G. Rand,et al. Lazy, not biased: Susceptibility to partisan fake news is better explained by lack of reasoning than by motivated reasoning , 2019, Cognition.
[8] David G. Rand,et al. The online laboratory: conducting experiments in a real labor market , 2010, ArXiv.
[9] Chanthika Pornpitakpan. The Persuasiveness of Source Credibility: A Critical Review of Five Decades' Evidence , 2004 .
[10] Adam J. Berinsky,et al. Processing political misinformation: comprehending the Trump phenomenon , 2017, Royal Society Open Science.
[11] A. Coppock. Generalizing from Survey Experiments Conducted on Mechanical Turk: A Replication Approach , 2018, Political Science Research and Methods.
[12] Matthew A. Baum,et al. Shot by the Messenger: Partisan Cues and Public Opinion Regarding National Security and War , 2009 .
[13] Thomas J. Leeper,et al. The Generalizability of Survey Experiments* , 2015, Journal of Experimental Political Science.
[14] Oliver A. McClellan,et al. Validating the demographic, political, psychological, and experimental results obtained from a new source of online survey respondents , 2019, Research & Politics.
[15] Adam Seth Levine,et al. Cross-Sample Comparisons and External Validity , 2014, Journal of Experimental Political Science.
[16] David G. Rand,et al. The Implied Truth Effect: Attaching Warnings to a Subset of Fake News Headlines Increases Perceived Accuracy of Headlines Without Warnings , 2019, Manag. Sci..
[17] Mor Naaman,et al. The Role of Source, Headline and Expressive Responding in Political News Evaluation , 2018 .
[18] E. Austin,et al. Source v. Content Effects on Judgments of News Believability , 1994 .
[19] Miriam J. Metzger,et al. The science of fake news , 2018, Science.
[20] David G. Rand,et al. Will the Crowd Game the Algorithm?: Using Layperson Judgments to Combat Misinformation on Social Media by Downranking Distrusted Sources , 2020, CHI.
[21] S. Sundar. The MAIN Model : A Heuristic Approach to Understanding Technology Effects on Credibility , 2007 .
[22] Adam J. Berinsky,et al. Rumors and Health Care Reform: Experiments in Political Misinformation , 2015, British Journal of Political Science.