Signature Movements Lead to Efficient Search for Threatening Actions

The ability to find and evade fighting persons in a crowd is potentially life-saving. To investigate how the visual system processes threatening actions, we employed a visual search paradigm with threatening boxer targets among emotionally-neutral walker distractors, and vice versa. We found that a boxer popped out for both intact and scrambled actions, whereas walkers did not. A reverse correlation analysis revealed that observers' responses clustered around the time of the “punch", a signature movement of boxing actions, but not around specific movements of the walker. These findings support the existence of a detector for signature movements in action perception. This detector helps in rapidly detecting aggressive behavior in a crowd, potentially through an expedited (sub)cortical threat-detection mechanism.

[1]  J. Cutting,et al.  Recognizing the sex of a walker from a dynamic point-light display , 1977 .

[2]  J. Cutting,et al.  Recognizing friends by their walk: Gait perception without familiarity cues , 1977 .

[3]  U. Bellugi,et al.  Perception of American sign language in dynamic point-light displays. , 1981, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[4]  A Treisman,et al.  Feature analysis in early vision: evidence from search asymmetries. , 1988, Psychological review.

[5]  C. H. Hansen,et al.  Finding the face in the crowd: an anger superiority effect. , 1988, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[6]  V. S. Ramachandran,et al.  Perception of shape from shading , 1988, Nature.

[7]  김제중 Biological Motion , 1990, Lecture Notes in Biomathematics.

[8]  W. Dittrich Action Categories and the Perception of Biological Motion , 1993, Perception.

[9]  P Cavanagh,et al.  Familiarity and pop-out in visual search , 1994, Perception & psychophysics.

[10]  G. Mather,et al.  Gender discrimination in biological motion displays based on dynamic cues , 1994, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[11]  S. Lea,et al.  Perception of Emotion from Dynamic Point-Light Displays Represented in Dance , 1996, Perception.

[12]  D. Purcell,et al.  It Takes a Confounded Face to Pop Out of a Crowd , 1996, Perception.

[13]  K. Nakayama,et al.  Robust representations for faces: evidence from visual search. , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[14]  A. Ohman,et al.  The face in the crowd revisited: a threat advantage with schematic stimuli. , 2001, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[15]  P. Cavanagh,et al.  Attention-based visual routines: sprites , 2001, Cognition.

[16]  A. Anderson,et al.  Lesions of the human amygdala impair enhanced perception of emotionally salient events , 2001, Nature.

[17]  J. Wolfe Asymmetries in visual search: An introduction , 2001, Perception & psychophysics.

[18]  A. Ohman,et al.  Emotion drives attention: detecting the snake in the grass. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[19]  M. Lappe,et al.  Measurement of generalization fields for the recognition of biological motion , 2002, Vision Research.

[20]  M. Graziano,et al.  Complex Movements Evoked by Microstimulation of Precentral Cortex , 2002, Neuron.

[21]  Arne Öhman,et al.  The Malicious Serpent , 2003 .

[22]  J. Norman,et al.  Aging and the perception of biological motion. , 2004, Psychology and aging.

[23]  P. Vuilleumier,et al.  How brains beware: neural mechanisms of emotional attention , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[24]  Kazuo Hiraki,et al.  Visual search for biological motion: An event-related potential study , 2006, Neuroscience Letters.

[25]  F. Pollick,et al.  A motion capture library for the study of identity, gender, and emotion perception from biological motion , 2006, Behavior research methods.

[26]  M. Shiffrar,et al.  The visual analysis of emotional actions , 2006, Social neuroscience.

[27]  N. Troje,et al.  The Inversion Effect in Biological Motion Perception: Evidence for a “Life Detector”? , 2006, Current Biology.

[28]  R. Blake,et al.  Perception of human motion. , 2007, Annual review of psychology.

[29]  Emily D Grossman,et al.  Temporal "Bubbles" reveal key features for point-light biological motion perception. , 2008, Journal of vision.

[30]  J. Deloache,et al.  Detecting the Snake in the Grass , 2008, Psychological science.

[31]  Dorita H. F. Chang,et al.  Perception of animacy and direction from local biological motion signals. , 2008, Journal of vision.

[32]  M. Thirkettle,et al.  Contributions of form, motion and task to biological motion perception. , 2009, Journal of vision.

[33]  Claire L. Roether,et al.  Critical features for the perception of emotion from gait. , 2009, Journal of vision.

[34]  R. Vogels,et al.  Functional differentiation of macaque visual temporal cortical neurons using a parametric action space. , 2009, Cerebral cortex.

[35]  M. Tamietto,et al.  Neural bases of the non-conscious perception of emotional signals , 2010, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[36]  Sheng He,et al.  Searching for Life Motion Signals , 2010, Psychological science.

[37]  Cristina Becchio,et al.  Inferring intentions from biological motion: A stimulus set of point-light communicative interactions , 2010, Behavior research methods.

[38]  Guy Wallis,et al.  The face-in-the-crowd effect: when angry faces are just cross(es). , 2011, Journal of vision.

[39]  G. McCarthy,et al.  The Wolfpack Effect , 2010, Psychological science.

[40]  L. Pessoa,et al.  Emotion processing and the amygdala: from a 'low road' to 'many roads' of evaluating biological significance , 2010, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.