Auditory information is beneficial for adults with Down syndrome in a continuous bimanual task.

Much recent research using discrete unimanual tasks has indicated that individuals with Down syndrome (DS) have more difficulty performing verbal-motor tasks as compared to visual-motor tasks (see Perceptual-Motor Behavior in Down Syndrome, Human Kinetics, Champaign, IL, 2000, p. 305 for a review). In continuous tasks, however, individuals with DS perform better when movement is guided by auditory information compared to visual information (Downs Syndr.: Res. Prac. 4 (1996) 25; J. Sport Exercise Psy. 22 (2000) S90). The aim of the present study was to investigate if there are any differences for adults with DS between visual, auditory and verbal guidance in a continuous bimanual task. Ten adults with DS, 10 adults without DS and 10 typically developing children drew lines bimanually towards the body (down) and away from the body (up) following three different guidance conditions: visual (flashing line), auditory (high tone, low tone), and verbal ("up", "down"). All participants produced mostly in-phase movements and were close to the 1000 ms target time for all guidance conditions. The adults with DS, however, displayed greater variability in their movement time, movement amplitude and bimanual coordination than adults without DS. For all groups, the left hand was slower and more variable in producing the lateral movements than the right hand. The results regarding guidance information suggest that auditory information is beneficial for repetitive bimanual tasks for adults with DS. Possible mechanisms that cause these results will be discussed.

[1]  M. Latash,et al.  What are “normal movements” in atypical populations? , 1996, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[2]  Robert B. Welch,et al.  Contributions of audition and vision to temporal rate perception , 1986, Perception & psychophysics.

[3]  U Bellugi,et al.  Callosal morphology concurs with neurobehavioral and neuropathological findings in two neurodevelopmental disorders. , 1992, Archives of neurology.

[4]  C. B. Walter,et al.  The coordination of limb movements with different kinematic patterns , 1988, Brain and Cognition.

[5]  D. Elliott,et al.  Asymmetries in intermanual transfer of training and motor overflow in adults with Down's syndrome and nonhandicapped children. , 1989, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[6]  Romeo Chua,et al.  A functional systems approach to understanding verbal-motor integration in individuals with Down syndrome , 1996 .

[7]  Walter E. Davis,et al.  Fractionated Reaction Times and Movement Times of Down Syndrome and Other Adults with Mental Retardation , 1991 .

[8]  Romeo Chua,et al.  Perceptual Motor Behavior in Down Syndrome , 2000 .

[9]  S P Swinnen,et al.  Spatial interactions during bimanual coordination patterns: the effect of directional compatibility. , 2001, Motor control.

[10]  R. Nicoletti,et al.  Is interhemispheric transfer of visuomotor information asymmetric? Evidence from a meta-analysis , 1991, Neuropsychologia.

[11]  D. Elliott,et al.  Cerebral Specialization for Speech Production in Persons with Down Syndrome , 1999, Brain and Language.

[12]  S. Swinnen,et al.  Acquiring bimanual skills: contrasting forms of information feedback for interlimb decoupling , 1993 .

[13]  Paula Fitzpatrick,et al.  Dynamical Patterns in the Development of Clapping. , 1996 .

[14]  R. C. Oldfield The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.

[15]  D. Elliott,et al.  Anomalous Cerebral Lateralization and Down Syndrome , 1994, Brain and Cognition.

[16]  H. Haken,et al.  A theoretical model of phase transitions in human hand movements , 2004, Biological Cybernetics.

[17]  C D Frith,et al.  Specific motor disabilities in Down's syndrome. , 1974, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[18]  Didier Cros,et al.  Physiological motor asymmetry in human handedness: evidence from transcranial magnetic stimulation , 1994, Brain Research.

[19]  X. Hartley Lateralisation of Speech Stimuli in Young Down's Syndrome Children , 1981, Cortex.

[20]  J. Summers,et al.  The Dynamics of Bimanual Circle Drawing , 1997, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[21]  D. Elliott,et al.  Manual and oral praxis in adults with Down's syndrome , 1990, Neuropsychologia.

[22]  Raja Parasuraman,et al.  Varieties of attention , 1984 .

[23]  D Elliott,et al.  Manual asymmetries in the performance of sequential movement by adolescents and adults with Down syndrome. , 1985, American journal of mental deficiency.

[24]  Daniel Cattaert,et al.  Hand coordination in bimanual circle drawing. , 1995 .

[25]  H. Zelaznik,et al.  Correlations for timing consistency among tapping and drawing tasks: evidence against a single timing process for motor control. , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[26]  D. Elliott,et al.  Cerebral specialization in young adults with Down syndrome. , 1987, American journal of mental deficiency.

[27]  M. Gazzaniga,et al.  Dissociation of Spatial and Temporal Coupling in the Bimanual Movements of Callosotomy Patients , 1996 .

[28]  Stephan P Swinnen,et al.  Exploring interlimb constraints during bimanual graphic performance: effects of muscle grouping and direction , 1998, Behavioural Brain Research.

[29]  D. H. Warren,et al.  Immediate perceptual response to intersensory discrepancy. , 1980, Psychological bulletin.

[30]  Natalia Dounskaia,et al.  Egocentric and Allocentric Constraints in the Expression of Patterns of Interlimb Coordination , 1997, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[31]  Will Spijkers,et al.  Structural Constraints on the Performance of Symmetrical Bimanual Movements with Different Amplitudes , 1995 .

[32]  Digby Elliott,et al.  Visual Feedback Processing and Goal-Directed Movement in Adults with Down Syndrome , 1995 .

[33]  S. Robertson,et al.  Development of Bimanual Skill: The Search for Stable Patterns of Coordination , 2001, Journal of motor behavior.

[34]  J. Kelso,et al.  Symmetry breaking dynamics of human multilimb coordination. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[35]  H N Zelaznik,et al.  Spatial topological constraints in a bimanual task. , 1991, Acta psychologica.

[36]  S. Swinnen,et al.  The organization of patterns of multilimb coordination as revealed through reaction time measures , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[37]  J. Kelso,et al.  Evolution of behavioral attractors with learning: nonequilibrium phase transitions. , 1992 .

[38]  S. Swinnen,et al.  Between-limb asynchronies during bimanual coordination: Effects of manual dominance and attentional cueing , 1996, Neuropsychologia.

[39]  D. Kahneman,et al.  Attention and Effort , 1973 .

[40]  R. Tannock,et al.  Do Individuals with Down's Syndrome Possess Right Hemisphere Language Dominance? , 1984, Cortex.

[41]  M. Wyke,et al.  The effects of brain lesions on the performance of bilateral arm movements. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.

[42]  X. Hartley Receptive language processing of Down's syndrome children. , 2008, Journal of mental deficiency research.

[43]  D Elliott,et al.  Lateral asymmetries in finger-tapping by adolescents and young adults with Down syndrome. , 1986, American journal of mental deficiency.

[44]  Natalia Dounskaia,et al.  Systematic error in the organization of physical action , 2001, Cogn. Sci..

[45]  R. Chua,et al.  Bimanual coordination dynamics in adults with Down syndrome. , 2002, Motor control.

[46]  D. Elliott,et al.  Cerebral Specialization for Speech Perception and Movement Organization in Adults with Down's Syndrome , 1993, Cortex.

[47]  D Elliott,et al.  Cerebral specialization and the control of oral and limb movements for individuals with Down's syndrome. , 1990, Journal of motor behavior.