Oscillatory correlates of intentional updating in episodic memory

The efficient use of our memory does not only require remembering encoded information, it also requires forgetting old out-of-date information. That such memory updating is part of our memory system is suggested by numerous behavioral studies. The physiological correlates of this process, however, still remain elusive. In this study we explore oscillatory correlates of memory updating as they occur in list-method directed forgetting. In this task, subjects are cued to forget a previously learned word list and to learn a new list of words instead. Such cuing typically leads to forgetting of the first list (List 1) and to memory enhancement of the second (List 2). Measuring EEGs during List-2 encoding, we identified two effects of the forget cue on oscillatory function: an increase in upper alpha power and a reduction in upper alpha phase coupling (11 to 13 Hz). Median-split analyses revealed that the two oscillatory effects were selectively related to the two behavioral effects. Whereas the increase in power was related to List-2 enhancement, the reduced phase coupling was related to List-1 forgetting. Our results point to separate neural origins of forgetting and enhancement and show that alpha oscillations play a critical role in intentional updating of episodic memory.

[1]  G. Radvansky,et al.  Studies of directed forgetting in older adults. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[2]  R. Oostenveld,et al.  Theta and Gamma Oscillations Predict Encoding and Retrieval of Declarative Memory , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[3]  Marc W Howard,et al.  Theta and Gamma Oscillations during Encoding Predict Subsequent Recall , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[4]  Peter F. Delaney,et al.  Can encoding differences explain the benefits of directed forgetting in the list-method paradigm? , 2003 .

[5]  C. Gerloff,et al.  Inhibitory control of acquired motor programmes in the human brain. , 2002, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[6]  Robert A. Bjork,et al.  Positive forgetting: The noninterference of Items intentionally forgotten , 1970 .

[7]  Colleen M. Kelley,et al.  A contextual change account of the directed forgetting effect. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[8]  Niels Birbaumer,et al.  Cross-frequency phase synchronization: A brain mechanism of memory matching and attention , 2008, NeuroImage.

[9]  C. Elger,et al.  Human memory formation is accompanied by rhinal–hippocampal coupling and decoupling , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[10]  Per B. Sederberg,et al.  Oscillatory correlates of the primacy effect in episodic memory , 2006, NeuroImage.

[11]  Bernhard Pastötter,et al.  The crucial role of postcue encoding in directed forgetting and context-dependent forgetting. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[12]  R. Bjork Retrieval inhibition as an adaptive mechanism in human memory. , 1989 .

[13]  Katherine Kipp,et al.  The Development of Efficient Inhibition: Evidence from Directed-Forgetting Tasks. , 1998 .

[14]  Christoph Braun,et al.  Coherence of gamma-band EEG activity as a basis for associative learning , 1999, Nature.

[15]  W. Klimesch,et al.  EEG alpha oscillations: The inhibition–timing hypothesis , 2007, Brain Research Reviews.

[16]  Michael C. Anderson,et al.  Suppressing unwanted memories by executive control , 2001, Nature.

[17]  P Rappelsberger,et al.  Long-range EEG synchronization during word encoding correlates with successful memory performance. , 2000, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[18]  K. Bäuml,et al.  Inhibitory deficits in older adults: list-method directed forgetting revisited. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[19]  P. F. Delaney,et al.  Directed forgetting in incidental learning and recognition testing: support for a two-factor account. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[20]  R. Blair,et al.  An alternative method for significance testing of waveform difference potentials. , 1993, Psychophysiology.

[21]  R E Geiselman,et al.  Disrupted retrieval in directed forgetting: a link with posthypnotic amnesia. , 1983, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[22]  W. Freiwald,et al.  Oscillatory synchrony in the monkey temporal lobe correlates with performance in a visual short-term memory task. , 2004, Cerebral cortex.

[23]  Jonathan M. Golding,et al.  Intentional forgetting: Interdisciplinary approaches. , 1998 .

[24]  Christopher Summerfield,et al.  Coherent theta-band EEG activity predicts item-context binding during encoding , 2005, NeuroImage.

[25]  Naoyuki Sato,et al.  Theta synchronization networks emerge during human object–place memory encoding , 2007, Neuroreport.

[26]  Simon Hanslmayr,et al.  Prestimulus oscillations predict visual perception performance between and within subjects , 2007, NeuroImage.

[27]  F. Varela,et al.  Perception's shadow: long-distance synchronization of human brain activity , 1999, Nature.

[28]  Jan Noyes,et al.  The disruption and dissolution of directed forgetting : Inhibitory control of memory , 2000 .

[29]  M. Conway,et al.  Disruption of Inhibitory Control of Memory Following Lesions to the Frontal and Temporal Lobes , 2003, Cortex.

[30]  J. Polich Updating P300: An integrative theory of P3a and P3b , 2007, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[31]  John J. Foxe,et al.  Increases in alpha oscillatory power reflect an active retinotopic mechanism for distracter suppression during sustained visuospatial attention. , 2006, Journal of neurophysiology.

[32]  G. Pfurtscheller,et al.  Event-related synchronization (ERS) in the alpha band--an electrophysiological correlate of cortical idling: a review. , 1996, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[33]  Colleen M. Kelley,et al.  Self-evaluation as a moderating factor of strategy change in directed forgetting benefits , 2004, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[34]  Celia B. Harris,et al.  Directed forgetting of recently recalled autobiographical memories. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[35]  J. Fell,et al.  Memory formation by neuronal synchronization , 2006, Brain Research Reviews.

[36]  W. Klimesch,et al.  Upper alpha ERD and absolute power: their meaning for memory performance. , 2006, Progress in brain research.

[37]  W. Klimesch EEG alpha and theta oscillations reflect cognitive and memory performance: a review and analysis , 1999, Brain Research Reviews.

[38]  Á. Pascual-Leone,et al.  α-Band Electroencephalographic Activity over Occipital Cortex Indexes Visuospatial Attention Bias and Predicts Visual Target Detection , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[39]  D. Tucker,et al.  EEG coherency. I: Statistics, reference electrode, volume conduction, Laplacians, cortical imaging, and interpretation at multiple scales. , 1997, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[40]  A. Benjamin The effects of list-method directed forgetting on recognition memory , 2006, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[41]  Brice A. Kuhl,et al.  Neural Systems Underlying the Suppression of Unwanted Memories , 2004, Science.

[42]  W. Klimesch,et al.  Theta band power in the human scalp EEG and the encoding of new information , 1996, Neuroreport.

[43]  Lili Sahakyan,et al.  The influence of directional associations on directed forgetting and interference. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[44]  F. Varela,et al.  Measuring phase synchrony in brain signals , 1999, Human brain mapping.

[45]  Kimron Shapiro,et al.  Modulation of long-range neural synchrony reflects temporal limitations of visual attention in humans. , 2004, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[46]  Craig J. Brozinsky,et al.  Oscillatory EEG correlates of episodic trace decay. , 2006, Cerebral cortex.

[47]  W. Ray,et al.  EEG alpha activity reflects attentional demands, and beta activity reflects emotional and cognitive processes. , 1985, Science.

[48]  A. Milne,et al.  On the regulation of recollection: The intentional forgetting of stereotypical memories , 1997 .

[49]  P. König,et al.  Top-down processing mediated by interareal synchronization. , 2000, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[50]  K. K. Harnishfeger,et al.  Intending to forget: the development of cognitive inhibition in directed forgetting. , 1996, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[51]  D. LaBerge,et al.  The modification of short-term memory through instructions to forget , 1968 .