An auditory analogue of the sperling partial report procedure: Evidence for brief auditory storage

Abstract Three experiments are reported on the partial report of material presented auditorily over three spatially different channels. When partial report was required by spatial location, it was superior to whole report if the cue came less than four seconds after the end of the stimuli (Exp. I). When partial report was required by semantic category (letters/digits) the relation between it and whole report depended on whether the S was asked also to attribute each item to its correct spatial location. When location was required partial report was lower than whole report and showed no significant decay with delay of the partial report indicator (Exp. II), but when location was not required, partial report was superior to whole report for indicator delays of less than two seconds (Exp. III). This superiority was, however, much less than that found in Exp. I when partial report was required by spatial location. These results are compatible with a store which has a useful life of around two seconds and from which material may be retrieved more easily by spatial location than by semantic category.

[1]  Franklin S. Cooper,et al.  Computer‐Controlled PCM System for Investigation of Dichotic Speech Perception , 1969 .

[2]  M I Posner,et al.  Chronometric analysis of classification. , 1967, Psychological review.

[3]  C. Eriksen,et al.  Temporal course of selective attention. , 1969, Journal of experimental psychology.

[4]  S. E. Clark,et al.  Retrieval of color information from preperceptual memory. , 1969, Journal of experimental psychology.

[5]  D. Norman Memory While Shadowing , 1969, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[6]  Robert G. Crowder,et al.  Improved recall for digits with delayed recall cues. , 1969 .

[7]  George Sperling,et al.  The information available in brief visual presentations. , 1960 .

[8]  Anne Treisman,et al.  Monitoring and storage of irrelevant messages in selective attention , 1964 .

[9]  Earl Hunt,et al.  What kind of computer is man , 1971 .

[10]  R. Haber Information-processing approaches to visual perception , 1969 .

[11]  J. V. von Wright Selection in visual immediate memory. , 1968, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[12]  M. Bryden,et al.  Attentional strategies and short-term memory in dichotic listening☆ , 1971 .

[13]  M. Turvey Repetition and the preperceptual information store. , 1967, Journal of experimental psychology.

[14]  Sandra Ethlyn Clark,et al.  Retrieval of color information from the preperceptual storage system , 1967 .

[15]  William G. Chase,et al.  Short-term visual storage , 1967 .

[16]  D. J. Murray,et al.  Attention and storage in dichotic listening. , 1969, Journal of experimental psychology.

[17]  G. Peak,et al.  Inverse forgetting in short-term memory. , 1966, Journal of experimental psychology.

[18]  E. Averbach,et al.  Short-term memory in vision , 1961 .

[19]  M. Eden,et al.  Note on Short Term Storage of Information in Vision , 1964, Perceptual and motor skills.

[20]  S. Glucksberg,et al.  Memory for nonattended auditory material , 1970 .

[21]  Michael T. Turvey,et al.  Retrieval from iconic memory with shape as the selection criterion , 1970 .

[22]  Bela Julesz,et al.  Lower Limits of Auditory Periodicity Analysis , 1963 .

[23]  R. G. Crowder,et al.  Precategorical acoustic storage (PAS) , 1969 .

[24]  John Morton,et al.  Absence of a Cross-Modal “Suffix Effect” in Short-Term Memory , 1970 .

[25]  D. Aaronson Temporal course of perception in an immediate recall task. , 1968 .

[26]  N. Moray,et al.  EXPERIMENTS ON THE FOUR-EARED MAN. , 1965, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.