Learning a route using a map and/or description in young and older adults

The study investigates age-related differences between young and older adults in environment learning. A sample of young adults (aged 20–30 years) and older adults (aged 60–70 years) was asked to learn a route under three conditions: using a map (M), a description (D), or a map plus a description (M + D). Results showed that spatial recall performance, measured with map drawing and pointing tasks, was higher under M and M + D conditions, than under D condition. Only in the map drawing task were age-related differences modulated by the learning conditions: Young adults performed better than older adults in D, but the two groups did not differ under M or M + D conditions. In contrast, independently of age group and learning condition, participants in the pointing task made fewer errors in aligned pointing compared to counteraligned pointing, and the performance was determined by their reasoning ability. Correlation analyses indicated a differential role played by age and fluid ability depending on the learning modality and the recall task considered. Overall, our findings suggest that in environment learning, the choice of input and the type of recall task modulate age-related differences between young and older adults.

[1]  P. Johnson-Laird,et al.  Mental Models: Towards a Cognitive Science of Language, Inference, and Consciousness , 1985 .

[2]  Michael Tlauka,et al.  Orientation specificity occurs in both small- and large-scale imagined routes presented as verbal descriptions. , 1999 .

[3]  M. Just,et al.  Constructing mental models of machines from text and diagrams. , 1993 .

[4]  Timothy P. McNamara,et al.  Systems of Spatial Reference in Human Memory , 2001, Cognitive Psychology.

[5]  Gabriel A. Radvansky,et al.  Aging and situation model processing , 2007, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[6]  G W Evans,et al.  Cognitive mapping: knowledge of real-world distance and location information. , 1980, Journal of experimental psychology. Human learning and memory.

[7]  T. Salthouse,et al.  What needs to be explained to account for age-related effects on multiple cognitive variables? , 2003, Psychology and aging.

[8]  A. R. Dobbs,et al.  Age differences in the interpretation of misaligned "You-Are-Here" maps. , 1994, Journal of gerontology.

[9]  Jill M. Andrassy,et al.  The role of reader age and focus of attention in creating situation models from narratives. , 1997, The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences.

[10]  J. F. Herman,et al.  Mental manipulation of spatial information in young and elderly adults. , 1980 .

[11]  Barbara Hayes-Roth,et al.  Differences in spatial knowledge acquired from maps and navigation , 1982, Cognitive Psychology.

[12]  T. Salthouse The processing-speed theory of adult age differences in cognition. , 1996, Psychological review.

[13]  Rolf A. Zwaan,et al.  Aging and functional spatial relations in comprehension and memory. , 2003, Psychology and aging.

[14]  W. Rogers,et al.  Age-related differences in the acquisition, utilization, and extension of a spatial mental model. , 1999, The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences.

[15]  P. Baltes,et al.  Intellectual functioning in old and very old age: cross-sectional results from the Berlin Aging Study. , 1997, Psychology and aging.

[16]  Mary Hegarty,et al.  Spatial Memory of Real Environments, Virtual Environments, and Maps , 2004 .

[17]  Walter Kintsch,et al.  Propositional and situational representations of text , 1985 .

[18]  Valérie Gyselinck,et al.  Visuospatial memory and phonological loop in learning from multimedia , 2002 .

[19]  D. Uttal,et al.  Words and maps: developmental changes in mental models of spatial information acquired from descriptions and depictions. , 2006, Developmental science.

[20]  Francesca Pazzaglia,et al.  Text and Picture Integration in Comprehending and Memorizing Spatial Descriptions , 2008 .

[21]  R. Bakeman,et al.  Observing interaction: An introduction to sequential analysis, 2nd ed. , 1997 .

[22]  S. Peterson,et al.  Mental Representations of Maps and Verbal Descriptions: Evidence They May Affect Text Memory Differently , 1995 .

[23]  Wilhelmina C. Savenye,et al.  Why maps improve memory for text: The influence of structural information on working memory operations , 1993 .

[24]  P. Lipman,et al.  Age and gender differences in the effectiveness of map-like learning aids in memory for routes. , 1995, The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences.

[25]  Wendy A. Rogers,et al.  Long-Term Retention of a Spatial Mental Model for Younger and Older Adults , 2004, Experimental aging research.

[26]  Philip Smith Roger Bakeman John M. Gottman , 1987, Animal Behaviour.

[27]  Bart Rypma,et al.  Age differences in components of mental-rotation task performance , 1991 .

[28]  Holly A. Taylorandbarbaratversky Spatial Mental Models Derived from Survey and Route Descriptions , 1992 .

[29]  E. Tolman Cognitive maps in rats and men. , 1948, Psychological review.

[30]  Gabriel A Radvansky,et al.  Aging and integrating spatial mental models. , 2007, Psychology and aging.

[31]  S. Ferris,et al.  Age‐associated memory impairment: Proposed diagnostic criteria and measures of clinical change — report of a national institute of mental health work group , 1986 .

[32]  Paul N Wilson,et al.  Influences on the First-Perspective Alignment Effect from Text Route Descriptions , 2008, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[33]  A. Glenberg,et al.  Comprehension of illustrated text: Pictures help to build mental models☆ , 1992 .

[34]  Francesca Pazzaglia,et al.  The role of mental rotation and age in spatial perspective-taking tasks: when age does not impair perspective-taking performance , 2006 .

[35]  Anthony E. Richardson,et al.  Spatial knowledge acquisition from maps and from navigation in real and virtual environments , 1999, Memory & cognition.

[36]  H. Oostendorp,et al.  The Construction of Mental Representations During Reading , 1998 .

[37]  Mary Hegarty,et al.  When static media promote active learning: annotated illustrations versus narrated animations in multimedia instruction. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Applied.

[38]  Clark C. Presson,et al.  Orientation Specificity in Representations of Place , 1997 .

[39]  Amy L Shelton,et al.  Individual skill differences and large-scale environmental learning. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[40]  Stephanie M. Clancy Dollinger,et al.  Mental rotation performance: Age, sex, and visual field differences , 1995 .

[41]  S. Vandenberg,et al.  Mental Rotations, a Group Test of Three-Dimensional Spatial Visualization , 1978, Perceptual and motor skills.

[42]  Gerard Hanley,et al.  The Placement and Misplacement of You-Are-Here Maps , 1984 .

[43]  Roberta L Klatzky,et al.  Functional equivalence of spatial representations derived from vision and language: evidence from allocentric judgments. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[44]  Elizabeth A L Stine-Morrow,et al.  Aging and the representation of spatial situations in narrative understanding. , 2002, The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences.

[45]  Tad T. Brunyé,et al.  Working memory in developing and applying mental models from spatial descriptions , 2008 .

[46]  G. Bower,et al.  The metrics of spatial situation models. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[47]  K. Meguro,et al.  Discrepancy Between Mental Rotation and Perspective-Taking Abilities in Normal Aging Assessed by Piaget's Three-Mountain Task , 2002, Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology.

[48]  Valérie Gyselinck,et al.  The role of illustrations in text comprehension: What, when, for whom, and why? , 1999 .

[49]  Uzma Ali,et al.  Intellectual Functioning of Individuals with Mental Disorders on Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale -Revised (WAIS -R) , 2011 .

[50]  M. G. Jones,et al.  Age-related differences in an ecologically based study of route learning. , 1997, Psychology and aging.

[51]  S. Kosslyn,et al.  Mental imagery and aging. , 1994, Psychology and aging.

[52]  I. Mammarella,et al.  Do working memory and susceptibility to interference predict individual differences in fluid intelligence? , 2006 .

[53]  Chiara Meneghetti,et al.  Working memory components and imagery instructions in the elaboration of a spatial mental model , 2007, Psychological research.

[54]  P. Ghisletta,et al.  Age differences in text processing: the role of working memory, inhibition, and processing speed. , 2011, The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences.

[55]  M. C. Newman,et al.  Older Adults Map Novel Environments but Do Not Place Learn: Findings From a Computerized Spatial Task , 2002 .

[56]  G. Bower,et al.  Mental models in narrative comprehension. , 1990, Science.

[57]  T. McNamara,et al.  Orientation and perspective dependence in route and survey learning. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.