Eye and Head Movements of Elite Baseball Players in Real Batting

In baseball, batters swing in response to a ball moving at high speed within a limited amount of time—about 0. 5 s. In order to make such movement possible, quick and accurate trajectory prediction followed by accurate swing motion with optimal body-eye coordination is considered essential, but the mechanisms involved are not clearly understood. The present study aims to clarify the strategies of eye and head movements adopted by elite baseball batters in actual game situations. In our experiment, six current professional baseball batters faced former professional baseball pitchers in a scenario close to a real game (i.e., without the batters informed about pitch type in advance). We measured eye movements with a wearable eye-tracker and head movements and bat trajectories with an optical motion capture system while the batters hit. In the eye movement measurements, contrary to previous studies, we found distinctive predictive saccades directed toward the predicted trajectory, of which the first saccades were initiated approximately 80–220 ms before impact for all participants. Predictive saccades were initiated significantly later when batters knew the types of pitch in advance compared to when they did not. We also found that the best three batters started predictive saccades significantly later and tended to have fewer gaze-ball errors than the other three batters. This result suggests that top batters spend slightly more time obtaining visual information by delaying the initiation of saccades. Furthermore, although all batters showed positive correlations between bat location and head direction at the time of impact, the better batters showed no correlation between bat location and gaze direction at that time. These results raise the possibility of differences in the coding process for the location of bat-ball contact; namely, that top batters might utilize head direction to encode impact locations.

[1]  Chantal Bard,et al.  Stabilization of head and eyes on target as a factor in successful basketball shooting , 1986 .

[2]  Mark Shelhamer,et al.  Saccades exhibit abrupt transition between reactive and predictive; predictive saccade sequences have long-term correlations. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[3]  A. Fuchs,et al.  Prediction in the oculomotor system: smooth pursuit during transient disappearance of a visual target , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[4]  John M. Findlay,et al.  Spatial and temporal factors in the predictive generation of saccadic eye movements , 1981, Vision Research.

[5]  David E. Irwin,et al.  Modern mental chronometry , 1988, Biological Psychology.

[6]  Michael F. Land,et al.  From eye movements to actions: how batsmen hit the ball , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[7]  R U Muller,et al.  Head-direction cells recorded from the postsubiculum in freely moving rats. I. Description and quantitative analysis , 1990, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[8]  M F Land,et al.  The knowledge base of the oculomotor system. , 1997, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[9]  Karl R Gegenfurtner,et al.  Keep your eyes on the ball: smooth pursuit eye movements enhance prediction of visual motion. , 2011, Journal of neurophysiology.

[10]  J L Croft,et al.  Visual strategies of sub-elite cricket batsmen in response to different ball velocities. , 2010, Human movement science.

[11]  H. T. Blair,et al.  Anticipatory head direction signals in anterior thalamus: evidence for a thalamocortical circuit that integrates angular head motion to compute head direction , 1995, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[12]  A. Schütz,et al.  How eye movements improve vision and action – comment on Vickers , 2016 .

[13]  B. Abernethy,et al.  The Head Tracks and Gaze Predicts: How the World’s Best Batters Hit a Ball , 2013, PloS one.

[14]  E. Brenner,et al.  Fast Responses of the Human Hand to Changes in Target Position. , 1997, Journal of motor behavior.

[15]  E. Rolls,et al.  Head direction cells in the primate pre‐subiculum , 1999, Hippocampus.

[16]  A. Montagnini,et al.  Do we track what we see? Common versus independent processing for motion perception and smooth pursuit eye movements: A review , 2011, Vision Research.

[17]  J D Crawford,et al.  Spatial transformations for eye-hand coordination. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.

[18]  Miriam Spering,et al.  Eye movement accuracy determines natural interception strategies. , 2016, Journal of vision.

[19]  A. Bahill,et al.  Why can't batters keep their eyes on the ball? , 1984 .

[20]  Eileen Kowler,et al.  The effect of expectations on slow oculomotor control—IV. Anticipatory smooth eye movements depend on prior target motions , 1984, Vision Research.

[21]  B. Tatler,et al.  Looking and Acting: Vision and eye movements in natural behaviour , 2009 .

[22]  Aaron B. Zimmerman,et al.  A Method to Monitor Eye and Head Tracking Movements in College Baseball Players , 2014, Optometry and vision science : official publication of the American Academy of Optometry.

[23]  Jeff B. Pelz,et al.  Predictive eye movements in natural vision , 2011, Experimental Brain Research.

[24]  Jeffrey Taube,et al.  Head direction cells , 2009, Scholarpedia.

[25]  D. Newport,et al.  Transient natural convection in a conducting enclosure heated from above , 2013, J. Vis..

[26]  J. Taube,et al.  Head Direction Cell Activity in Mice: Robust Directional Signal Depends on Intact Otolith Organs , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[27]  Kazuyuki Kanosue,et al.  Head-eye movement of collegiate baseball batters during fastball hitting , 2018, PloS one.