Automatic effects of instructions do not require the intention to execute these instructions

ABSTRACT Prior research established that newly instructed stimulus-response mappings, which have never been executed overtly before, can lead to automatic response-congruency effects. Such instruction-based congruency effects have been taken as evidence for the hypothesis that the intention to execute stimulus-response mappings results into functional associations that serve future execution. The present study challenges this hypothesis by demonstrating in a series of four experiments that maintaining instructed stimulus-response mappings for future recognition, rather than for future execution, can also lead to an instruction-based congruency effect. These findings indicate that the instruction-based congruency effect emerges even when it is very unlikely that participants form the intention to execute instructions. Alternative interpretations of the instruction-based congruency effect are discussed.

[1]  Nachshon Meiran,et al.  The representation of instructions operates like a prepared reflex: flanker compatibility effects found in first trial following S-R instructions. , 2009, Experimental psychology.

[2]  Michael W. Cole,et al.  Reflexive activation of newly instructed stimulus–response rules: evidence from lateralized readiness potentials in no-go trials , 2015, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[3]  H. Munk Zur Physiologie der Grosshirnrinde , 1902 .

[4]  Marcel Brass,et al.  There is more into ‘doing’ than ‘knowing’: The function of the right inferior frontal sulcus is specific for implementing versus memorising verbal instructions , 2016, NeuroImage.

[5]  Steve Majerus,et al.  Human Neuroscience Hypothesis and Theory Article Finding the Answer in Space: the Mental Whiteboard Hypothesis on Serial Order in Working Memory , 2022 .

[6]  Christina U. Pfeuffer,et al.  The Power of Words: On Item-Specific Stimulus–Response Associations Formed in the Absence of Action , 2017, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[7]  Michael W. Cole,et al.  The power of instructions: Proactive configuration of stimulus-response translation. , 2015, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[8]  Learning through instructions vs. learning through practice: flanker congruency effects from instructed and applied S-R mappings , 2014, Psychological research.

[9]  Nachshon Meiran,et al.  The representation of instructions in working memory leads to autonomous response activation: Evidence from the first trials in the flanker paradigm , 2006, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[10]  Baptist Liefooghe,et al.  Automatic motor activation by mere instruction , 2014, Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience.

[11]  R. Gaschler,et al.  Instruction-induced feature binding , 2007, Psychological research.

[12]  Dorit Wenke,et al.  Strategic influences on implementing instructions for future actions , 2009, Psychological research.

[13]  Susan E. Gathercole,et al.  Working memory abilities and children's performance in laboratory analogues of classroom activities , 2008 .

[14]  Hannes Ruge,et al.  Rapid formation of pragmatic rule representations in the human brain during instruction-based learning. , 2010, Cerebral cortex.

[15]  Michael W. Cole,et al.  When planning results in loss of control: intention-based reflexivity and working-memory , 2012, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[16]  Frederick Verbruggen,et al.  Tscope: A C library for programming cognitive experiments on the MS Windows platform , 2006, Behavior research methods.

[17]  Christina U. Pfeuffer,et al.  Multiple priming instances increase the impact of practice-based but not verbal code-based stimulus-response associations. , 2017, Acta psychologica.

[18]  Benefit of enactment over oral repetition of verbal instruction does not require additional working memory during encoding , 2013, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[19]  N. Meiran Reconfiguration of processing mode prior to task performance. , 1996 .

[20]  R. Kliegl,et al.  Dissociating retention and access in working memory: An age-comparative study of mental arithmetic , 2001, Memory & cognition.

[21]  R. Hartsuiker,et al.  Timed picture naming norms for 590 pictures in Dutch. , 2005, Acta psychologica.

[22]  Steve Majerus,et al.  Spatial Attention Interacts With Serial-Order Retrieval From Verbal Working Memory , 2013, Psychological science.

[23]  J. G. Snodgrass,et al.  A standardized set of 260 pictures: norms for name agreement, image agreement, familiarity, and visual complexity. , 1980, Journal of experimental psychology. Human learning and memory.

[24]  Amanda H. Waterman,et al.  How does enactment affect the ability to follow instructions in working memory? , 2015, Memory & cognition.

[25]  Robert W. Proctor,et al.  Automaticity without extensive training: The role of memory retrieval in implementation of task-defined rules , 2011, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[26]  Stefaan Vandorpe,et al.  Further evidence for the role of mode-independent short-term associations in spatial Simon effects , 2005, Perception & psychophysics.

[27]  P. Barrouillet,et al.  Time constraints and resource sharing in adults' working memory spans. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[28]  G. D. Logan Task Switching , 2022 .

[29]  A. Kiesel,et al.  Task switching: on the origin of response congruency effects , 2007, Psychological research.

[30]  Yoav Kessler,et al.  The task rule congruency effect in task switching reflects activated long-term memory. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[31]  A. Vandierendonck,et al.  Task switching: interplay of reconfiguration and interference control. , 2010, Psychological bulletin.

[32]  Baptist Liefooghe,et al.  Eliminating the Simon effect by instruction. , 2014, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[33]  J. de Houwer,et al.  Instruction-based task-rule congruency effects. , 2012, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[34]  J. Houwer,et al.  Instruction-based response activation depends on task preparation , 2013, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[35]  Andrea M Philipp,et al.  Control and interference in task switching--a review. , 2010, Psychological bulletin.

[36]  M. Brass,et al.  It wasn’t me! Motor activation from irrelevant spatial information in the absence of a response , 2015, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[37]  Christina U. Pfeuffer,et al.  Defining stimulus representation in stimulus–response associations formed on the basis of task execution and verbal codes , 2018, Psychological research.

[38]  J. Houwer,et al.  Attention to future actions: the influence of instructed S-R versus S-S mappings on attentional control , 2016, Psychological research.

[39]  M. Brass,et al.  There Are Limits to the Effects of Task Instructions: Making the Automatic Effects of Task Instructions Context-Specific Takes Practice , 2017, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[40]  André Vandierendonck,et al.  A comparison of methods to combine speed and accuracy measures of performance: A rejoinder on the binning procedure , 2016, Behavior Research Methods.

[41]  Baptist Liefooghe,et al.  The role of motor imagery in learning via instructions. , 2017, Acta psychologica.

[42]  Jean-Philippe van Dijck,et al.  A working memory account for spatial–numerical associations , 2011, Cognition.

[43]  Baptist Liefooghe,et al.  Congruency effects on the basis of instructed response-effect contingencies. , 2015, Acta psychologica.

[44]  K. Oberauer Access to information in working memory: exploring the focus of attention. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[45]  Freya Acar,et al.  A Working Memory Account of the Interaction between Numbers and Spatial Attention , 2014, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[46]  Nachshon Meiran,et al.  Working memory load but not multitasking eliminates the prepared reflex: further evidence from the adapted flanker paradigm. , 2012, Acta psychologica.