Effects of social context and predictive relevance on action outcome monitoring

Outcome monitoring is crucial for subsequent adjustments in behavior and is associated with a specific electrophysiological response, the feedback-related negativity (FRN). Besides feedback generated by one’s own action, the performance of others may also be relevant for oneself, and the observation of outcomes for others’ actions elicits an observer FRN (oFRN). To test how these components are influenced by social setting and predictive value of feedback information, we compared event-related potentials, as well as their topographies and neural generators, for performance feedback generated by oneself and others in a cooperative versus competitive context. Our results show that (1) the predictive relevance of outcomes is crucial to elicit an FRN in both players and observers, (2) cooperation increases FRN and P300 amplitudes, especially in individuals with high traits of perspective taking, and (3) contrary to previous findings on gambling outcomes, oFRN components are generated for both cooperating and competing observers, but with smaller amplitudes in the latter. Neural source estimation revealed medial prefrontal activity for both FRN and oFRN, but with additional generators for the oFRN in the dorsolateral and ventral prefrontal cortex, as well as the temporoparietal junction. We conclude that the latter set of brain regions could mediate social influences on action monitoring by representing agency and social relevance of outcomes and are, therefore, recruited in addition to shared prediction error signals generated in medial frontal areas during action outcome observation.

[1]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Neurophysiological mechanisms underlying the understanding and imitation of action , 2001, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[2]  Jason P. Mitchell Activity in right temporo-parietal junction is not selective for theory-of-mind. , 2008, Cerebral cortex.

[3]  P. Rabbitt Errors and error correction in choice-response tasks. , 1966, Journal of experimental psychology.

[4]  H. Critchley,et al.  Neural systems supporting interoceptive awareness , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[5]  A. Sanfey,et al.  Independent Coding of Reward Magnitude and Valence in the Human Brain , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[6]  Giuseppe Sartori,et al.  When people matter more than money: An ERPs study , 2010, Brain Research Bulletin.

[7]  G. Pourtois Early Error Detection Predicted by Reduced Pre-response Control Process: An ERP Topographic Mapping Study , 2010, Brain Topography.

[8]  Christian Bellebaum,et al.  It was not MY fault: event-related brain potentials in active and observational learning from feedback. , 2010, Cerebral cortex.

[9]  Alison L. Chasteen,et al.  Your mistakes are mine: Self-other overlap predicts neural response to observed errors , 2010 .

[10]  C. N. Macrae,et al.  Finding the Self? An Event-Related fMRI Study , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[11]  Benoit Bediou,et al.  Delayed monitoring of accuracy errors compared to commission errors in ACC , 2012, NeuroImage.

[12]  M. Herrmann,et al.  Source localization (LORETA) of the error-related-negativity (ERN/Ne) and positivity (Pe). , 2004, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[13]  P. Liddle,et al.  External Behavior Monitoring Mirrors Internal Behavior Monitoring , 2005 .

[14]  Rebecca J. Compton,et al.  Perceived similarity and neural mirroring: Evidence from vicarious error processing , 2009, Social neuroscience.

[15]  P Berg,et al.  A multiple source approach to the correction of eye artifacts. , 1994, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[16]  Denis Brunet,et al.  Topographic ERP Analyses: A Step-by-Step Tutorial Review , 2008, Brain Topography.

[17]  Jason P. Mitchell,et al.  Dissociable Medial Prefrontal Contributions to Judgments of Similar and Dissimilar Others , 2006, Neuron.

[18]  M. Posner,et al.  Cognitive and emotional influences in anterior cingulate cortex , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[19]  John J. Foxe,et al.  The role of cingulate cortex in the detection of errors with and without awareness: a high‐density electrical mapping study , 2007, The European journal of neuroscience.

[20]  Clay B. Holroyd,et al.  It's worse than you thought: the feedback negativity and violations of reward prediction in gambling tasks. , 2007, Psychophysiology.

[21]  David W. Johnson,et al.  Effects of cooperative, competitive, and individualistic goal structures on achievement: A meta-analysis. , 1981 .

[22]  A. Mitchell Apoptosis: Jaws of death , 2001, Nature Reviews Molecular Cell Biology.

[23]  D. Lehmann,et al.  Segmentation of brain electrical activity into microstates: model estimation and validation , 1995, IEEE Transactions on Biomedical Engineering.

[24]  The effect of interpersonal competition on monitoring internal and external error feedback. , 2010, Psychophysiology.

[25]  Rongjun Yu,et al.  Brain potentials associated with outcome expectation and outcome evaluation , 2006, Neuroreport.

[26]  H. Bekkering,et al.  The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Your Mistake Is My Mistake . . . or Is It? Behavioural Adjustments following Own and Observed Actions in Cooperative and Competitive Contexts , 2022 .

[27]  Gilles Pourtois,et al.  Early error detection is generic, but subsequent adaption to errors is not: Evidence from ERPs , 2011, Neuropsychologia.

[28]  U. Wagner,et al.  Guilt-specific processing in the prefrontal cortex. , 2011, Cerebral cortex.

[29]  L. Festinger A Theory of Social Comparison Processes , 1954 .

[30]  Ralf H. Trippe,et al.  Parallel brain activity for self-generated and observed errors , 2004 .

[31]  J Mazziotta,et al.  A probabilistic atlas and reference system for the human brain: International Consortium for Brain Mapping (ICBM). , 2001, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[32]  Clay B. Holroyd,et al.  The neural basis of human error processing: reinforcement learning, dopamine, and the error-related negativity. , 2002, Psychological review.

[33]  Wim Fias,et al.  Outcome expectancy and not accuracy determines posterror slowing: ERP support , 2010, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[34]  J. Wallis Orbitofrontal cortex and its contribution to decision-making. , 2007, Annual review of neuroscience.

[35]  R. Malach,et al.  When the Brain Loses Its Self: Prefrontal Inactivation during Sensorimotor Processing , 2006, Neuron.

[36]  R Saxe,et al.  People thinking about thinking people The role of the temporo-parietal junction in “theory of mind” , 2003, NeuroImage.

[37]  D. Lehmann,et al.  Reference-free identification of components of checkerboard-evoked multichannel potential fields. , 1980, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[38]  C. Büchel,et al.  Neural representations of subjective reward value , 2010, Behavioural Brain Research.

[39]  Uta Frith,et al.  Theory of mind , 2001, Current Biology.

[40]  J. Hohnsbein,et al.  ERP components on reaction errors and their functional significance: a tutorial , 2000, Biological Psychology.

[41]  Xiaolin Zhou,et al.  Modulation of the brain activity in outcome evaluation by interpersonal relationship: An ERP study , 2010, Neuropsychologia.

[42]  Kazuo Hiraki,et al.  Whose loss is it? Human electrophysiological correlates of non-self reward processing , 2009, Social neuroscience.

[43]  W. Prinz,et al.  Representing others' actions: just like one's own? , 2003, Cognition.

[44]  N. Squires,et al.  Two varieties of long-latency positive waves evoked by unpredictable auditory stimuli in man. , 1975, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[45]  Josep Marco-Pallarés,et al.  When decisions of others matter to me: an electrophysiological analysis , 2010, BMC Neuroscience.

[46]  N. Triplett,et al.  The Dynamogenic Factors in Pacemaking and Competition , 1898 .

[47]  Jun'ichi Katayama,et al.  Self-relevant criteria determine the evaluation of outcomes induced by others , 2008, Neuroreport.

[48]  J. Hohnsbein,et al.  Effects of crossmodal divided attention on late ERP components. II. Error processing in choice reaction tasks. , 1991, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[49]  M. Brass,et al.  The hidden side of intentional action: the role of the anterior insular cortex , 2010, Brain Structure and Function.

[50]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  A critique of functional localisers , 2006, NeuroImage.

[51]  David I. Perrett,et al.  An fMRI study of joint attention experience , 2005, NeuroImage.

[52]  Christoph M. Michel,et al.  Two electrophysiological stages of spatial orienting towards fearful faces: early temporo-parietal activation preceding gain control in extrastriate visual cortex , 2005, NeuroImage.

[53]  M. Murray,et al.  EEG source imaging , 2004, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[54]  Thérèse J. M. Overbeek,et al.  Dissociable Components of Error Processing on the Functional Significance of the Pe Vis-à-vis the Ern/ne Performance Monitoring Processes Reflected in the Ne and Pe Review of Studies That Report Both Ne and Pe: Associations and Dissociations Pharmacological Effects , 2022 .

[55]  J. Tangney,et al.  Moral affect: the good, the bad, and the ugly. , 1991, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[56]  C. Neil Macrae,et al.  The Link between Social Cognition and Self-referential Thought in the Medial Prefrontal Cortex , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[57]  J. Decety,et al.  Neural mechanisms subserving the perception of human actions , 1999, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[58]  G. Pourtois,et al.  Anxiety not only increases, but also alters early error-monitoring functions , 2010, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[59]  M. Kringelbach The human orbitofrontal cortex: linking reward to hedonic experience , 2005, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[60]  Clay B. Holroyd,et al.  Brain potentials associated with expected and unexpected good and bad outcomes. , 2005, Psychophysiology.

[61]  Xiaolin Zhou,et al.  The P300 and reward valence, magnitude, and expectancy in outcome evaluation , 2009, Brain Research.

[62]  H. Bekkering,et al.  Modulation of activity in medial frontal and motor cortices during error observation , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[63]  Timothy Edward John Behrens,et al.  Contrasting roles for cingulate and orbitofrontal cortex in decisions and social behaviour , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[64]  H. Critchley,et al.  A common role of insula in feelings, empathy and uncertainty , 2009, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[65]  C. Carter,et al.  The Timing of Action-Monitoring Processes in the Anterior Cingulate Cortex , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[66]  K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al.  The Role of the Medial Frontal Cortex in Cognitive Control , 2004, Science.

[67]  C. Carver,et al.  The self-attention-induced feedback loop and social facilitation. , 1981 .

[68]  R D Pascual-Marqui,et al.  Standardized low-resolution brain electromagnetic tomography (sLORETA): technical details. , 2002, Methods and findings in experimental and clinical pharmacology.

[69]  F. Overwalle Social cognition and the brain: a meta-analysis. , 2009 .

[70]  Christoph M. Michel,et al.  Spatiotemporal Dynamics of Human Cognition. , 1999, News in physiological sciences : an international journal of physiology produced jointly by the International Union of Physiological Sciences and the American Physiological Society.

[71]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  A unifying view of the basis of social cognition , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[72]  W. Gehring,et al.  Neural Systems for Error Monitoring , 2007, The Neuroscientist : a review journal bringing neurobiology, neurology and psychiatry.

[73]  Sina Radke,et al.  Mistakes that affect others: An fMRI study on processing of own errors in a social context , 2011, Experimental Brain Research.

[74]  Jutta Stahl,et al.  Error detection and the use of internal and external error indicators: an investigation of the first-indicator hypothesis. , 2010, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[75]  M. Petrides Lateral prefrontal cortex: architectonic and functional organization , 2005, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[76]  S. Morand,et al.  Electric source imaging of human brain functions , 2001, Brain Research Reviews.

[77]  C. Summerfield,et al.  An information theoretical approach to prefrontal executive function , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[78]  Thomas F Münte,et al.  Internal and external information in error processing , 2008, BMC Neuroscience.

[79]  Clay B. Holroyd,et al.  The feedback-related negativity reflects the binary evaluation of good versus bad outcomes , 2006, Biological Psychology.

[80]  Gilles Pourtois,et al.  Unavoidable errors: A spatio-temporal analysis of time-course and neural sources of evoked potentials associated with error processing in a speeded task , 2008, Neuropsychologia.

[81]  Kazuo Hiraki,et al.  Perceiving an opponent's loss: gender-related differences in the medial-frontal negativity. , 2006, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[82]  Margot J. Taylor,et al.  Guidelines for using human event-related potentials to study cognition: recording standards and publication criteria. , 2000, Psychophysiology.

[83]  Mark H. Davis Measuring individual differences in empathy: Evidence for a multidimensional approach. , 1983 .

[84]  Markus Ullsperger,et al.  When Errors Are Rewarding , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[85]  S. Delplanque,et al.  Beyond Conventional Event-related Brain Potential (ERP): Exploring the Time-course of Visual Emotion Processing Using Topographic and Principal Component Analyses , 2008, Brain Topography.

[86]  Andrew D. Engell,et al.  The Neural Bases of Cognitive Conflict and Control in Moral Judgment , 2004, Neuron.

[87]  K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al.  Conscious perception of errors and its relation to the anterior insula , 2010, Brain Structure and Function.

[88]  D. Meyer,et al.  A Neural System for Error Detection and Compensation , 1993 .

[89]  Luca Passamonti,et al.  A Key Role for Similarity in Vicarious Reward , 2009, Science.

[90]  R. J. Dolan,et al.  Differential neural response to positive and negative feedback in planning and guessing tasks , 1997, Neuropsychologia.

[91]  J. Decety,et al.  How Would You Feel versus How Do You Think She Would Feel? A Neuroimaging Study of Perspective-Taking with Social Emotions , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[92]  M. Brass,et al.  Inhibition of imitative behaviour and social cognition , 2009, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[93]  J. Decety,et al.  From the perception of action to the understanding of intention , 2001, Nature reviews. Neuroscience.

[94]  R. Zajonc SOCIAL FACILITATION. , 1965, Science.

[95]  I. Daum,et al.  Learning‐related changes in reward expectancy are reflected in the feedback‐related negativity , 2008, The European journal of neuroscience.

[96]  Marcel Brass,et al.  Control of shared representations relies on key processes involved in mental state attribution , 2009, Human brain mapping.

[97]  M. Jeannerod Neural Simulation of Action: A Unifying Mechanism for Motor Cognition , 2001, NeuroImage.

[98]  Norbert Kathmann,et al.  Neural correlates of error awareness , 2007, NeuroImage.

[99]  A. Damasio,et al.  Emotion, decision making and the orbitofrontal cortex. , 2000, Cerebral cortex.

[100]  W. Fias,et al.  How monitoring other's actions influences one's own performance. , 2011, Experimental psychology.

[101]  C. Braun,et al.  Event-Related Brain Potentials Following Incorrect Feedback in a Time-Estimation Task: Evidence for a Generic Neural System for Error Detection , 1997, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[102]  The effect of interpersonal competition on monitoring internal and external error feedback. , 2010 .

[103]  Sterling C. Johnson,et al.  Metacognitive evaluation, self-relevance, and the right prefrontal cortex , 2004, NeuroImage.

[104]  E. Donchin,et al.  On quantifying surprise: the variation of event-related potentials with subjective probability. , 1977, Psychophysiology.

[105]  Maarten A. S. Boksem,et al.  Please Scroll down for Article Social Neuroscience Facing Disapproval: Performance Monitoring in a Social Context Facing Disapproval: Performance Monitoring in a Social Context Performance Monitoring in a Social Context , 2022 .

[106]  Leonie Koban,et al.  When your errors make me lose or win: Event-related potentials to observed errors of cooperators and competitors , 2010, Social neuroscience.

[107]  A. Damasio,et al.  Individuals with sociopathic behavior caused by frontal damage fail to respond autonomically to social stimuli , 1990, Behavioural Brain Research.

[108]  Peter Ullsperger,et al.  Dissociable medial frontal negativities from a common monitoring system for self- and externally caused failure of goal achievement , 2009, NeuroImage.

[109]  Adrian R. Willoughby,et al.  The Medial Frontal Cortex and the Rapid Processing of Monetary Gains and Losses , 2002, Science.

[110]  Jan R. Wiersema,et al.  Cascade of Neural Events Leading from Error Commission to Subsequent Awareness Revealed Using EEG Source Imaging , 2011, PloS one.

[111]  A. Buss,et al.  The aggression questionnaire. , 1992, Journal of personality and social psychology.