Learning of timing patterns and the development of temporal expectations

The present study investigated the learning of a culturally unfamiliar musical rhythm, leading to the development of temporal expectations, and it explored the potential for generalization across tempi and tasks. With that aim, we adapted the serial reaction time task to examine the learning of temporal structures by an indirect method. The temporal pattern employed was based on a complex interval ratio (2:3) and compared to one based on a simple interval ratio (1:2). In the exposure phase, non-musician participants performed a two-choice speeded discrimination task that required responding by key press to each event of the simple or complex auditory pattern. Participants were not informed about the temporal regularities; their task solely concerned the discrimination task. During exposure (Experiments 1–3), response times decreased over time for both temporal patterns, but particularly for the events following the longer interval of the more complex 2:3 pattern. Exposure further influenced performance in subsequent testing phases, notably the precision of tap timing in a production task (Experiment 2) and temporal expectations in a perception task (Experiment 3). Our findings promote the new paradigm introduced here as a method to investigate the learning of temporal structures.

[1]  M. Peters The relationship between variability of intertap intervals and interval duration , 1989 .

[2]  Matthew Flatt,et al.  PsyScope: An interactive graphic system for designing and controlling experiments in the psychology laboratory using Macintosh computers , 1993 .

[3]  Morten H. Christiansen,et al.  Synchronization and Continuation Tapping to Complex Meters , 2006 .

[4]  Axel Cleeremans,et al.  Cognitive Control of Sequential Knowledge in 2-Year-Olds , 2007, Psychological science.

[5]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Learning the structure of event sequences. , 1991, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[6]  E. Large,et al.  The dynamics of attending: How people track time-varying events. , 1999 .

[7]  Edgar Erdfelder,et al.  A Multinomial Model to Assess Fluency and Recollection in a Sequence Learning Task , 1997 .

[8]  A. Buchner,et al.  Simultaneous learning of different regularities in sequence learning tasks: limits and characteristics , 2001, Psychological research.

[9]  Peter Desain,et al.  The Formation of Rhythmic Categories and Metric Priming , 2003, Perception.

[10]  Erik D. Thiessen,et al.  When cues collide: use of stress and statistical cues to word boundaries by 7- to 9-month-old infants. , 2003, Developmental psychology.

[11]  R. Näätänen,et al.  Foreperiod and simple reaction time. , 1981 .

[12]  P. Fraisse The psychology of time , 1963 .

[13]  P. J. Foley The foreperiod and simple reaction time. , 1959, Canadian journal of psychology.

[14]  M. Jones,et al.  Effects of auditory pattern structure on anticipatory and reactive attending , 2006, Cognitive Psychology.

[15]  Aniruddh D. Patel,et al.  The influence of metricality and modality on synchronization with a beat , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.

[16]  P. Perruchet,et al.  2.32 Implicit Learning , 2022 .

[17]  Simon R. Hawkins,et al.  Imitation and production of interval ratios , 1986, Perception & psychophysics.

[18]  Andrew J. Rosenfeld,et al.  Perception and Production of Syncopated Rhythms , 2007 .

[19]  D. Shanks,et al.  Characteristics of dissociable human learning systems , 1994, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[20]  Thomas Bäck An Empirical Comparison , 1996 .

[21]  Justin London,et al.  Some Examples of Complex Meters and Their Implications for Models of Metric Perception , 1995 .

[22]  M. R. Jones,et al.  Dynamic attending and responses to time. , 1989, Psychological review.

[23]  Eric Soetens,et al.  Perceptual or motor learning in SRT tasks with complex sequence structures , 2006, Psychological research.

[24]  Barbara Tillmann,et al.  Influence of tonal and temporal expectations on chord processing and on completion judgments of chord sequences , 2006, Psychological research.

[25]  L. Cuddy,et al.  Detection of metric structure in auditory figural patterns , 2002, Perception & psychophysics.

[26]  Paul Fraisse,et al.  L'anticipation de stimulus rythmiques: Vitesse d'établissement et précision de la synchronisation , 1966 .

[27]  M. Nissen,et al.  Attentional requirements of learning: Evidence from performance measures , 1987, Cognitive Psychology.

[28]  B. Tillmann,et al.  Implicit learning of tonality: a self-organizing approach. , 2000, Psychological review.

[29]  J. Salidis,et al.  Nonconscious temporal cognition: Learning rhythms implicitly , 2001, Memory & cognition.

[30]  H. Roediger Cognitive psychology of memory , 2008 .

[31]  S. Trehub,et al.  Tuning in to musical rhythms: infants learn more readily than adults. , 2005, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[32]  E. Large Resonating to Musical Rhythm : Theory and Experiment , 2008 .

[33]  Saul Sternberg,et al.  TilDing by Skilled Musicians , 1982 .

[34]  S. Trehub,et al.  Metrical Categories in Infancy and Adulthood , 2005, Psychological science.

[35]  Richard B Ivry,et al.  Concurrent learning of temporal and spatial sequences. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[36]  D. Deutsch,et al.  The Psychology of Music , 1983 .

[37]  Aniruddh D. Patel,et al.  An empirical comparison of rhythm in language and music , 2003, Cognition.

[38]  D. Berry How implicit is implicit learning , 1997 .

[39]  G. Remillard Pure perceptual-based sequence learning. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[40]  S. Handel The differentiation of rhythmic structure , 1992, Perception & psychophysics.

[41]  Hannes Eisler,et al.  Psychophysical issues in the study of time perception. , 2008 .

[42]  M A Schmuckler,et al.  Harmonic and rhythmic influences on musical expectancy , 1994, Perception & psychophysics.

[43]  Simon Grondin,et al.  Psychology of Time , 2008 .

[44]  Peter Essens,et al.  Perception of Temporal Patterns , 1985 .

[45]  Gordon D. Logan,et al.  Hierarchical control of cognitive processes: switching tasks in sequences. , 2006 .

[46]  Emmanuel Bigand,et al.  The emergence of explicit knowledge during the early phase of learning in sequential reaction time tasks , 1997 .

[47]  Amandine Penel,et al.  Speeded Detection of a Tone Embedded in a Quasi-isochronous Sequence: Effects of a Task-Irrelevant Temporal Irregularity , 2005 .

[48]  Peder J. Johnson,et al.  Assessing implicit learning with indirect tests: Determining what is learned about sequence structure. , 1994 .

[49]  Bruno H. Repp,et al.  Variability of timing in expressive piano performance increases with interval duration , 1997 .

[50]  Peter E Keller,et al.  Staying offbeat: Sensorimotor syncopation with structured and unstructured auditory sequences , 2005, Psychological research.

[51]  Peter E. Keller,et al.  When Two Limbs are Weaker than One: Sensorimotor Syncopation with Alternating Hands , 2004, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[52]  G. Aschersleben Temporal Control of Movements in Sensorimotor Synchronization , 2002, Brain and Cognition.

[53]  C. Wright,et al.  Temporal rescaling of simple and complex ratios in rhythmic tapping. , 1995 .

[54]  B. Burns,et al.  Perceptual and motor factors in the imitation of simple temporal patterns , 1989, Psychological research.

[55]  Axel Cleeremans,et al.  Can sequence learning be implicit? New evidence with the process dissociation procedure , 2001, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[56]  Peter E. Keller,et al.  PRODUCTION AND SYNCHRONIZATION OF UNEVEN RHYTHMS AT FAST TEMPI , 2005 .

[57]  C. Schlier,et al.  The dependence of simple reaction time on temporal patterns of stimuli , 1972, Kybernetik.

[58]  C D Frith,et al.  On the benefits of not trying: brain activity and connectivity reflecting the interactions of explicit and implicit sequence learning. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.

[59]  Darryl W. Schneider,et al.  Hierarchical control of cognitive processes: switching tasks in sequences. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[60]  R. Kauffman African Rhythm: A Reassessment , 1980 .

[61]  D J Povel,et al.  Internal representation of simple temporal patterns. , 1981, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[62]  R B Ivry,et al.  The coupled oscillator model of between-hand coordination in alternate-hand tapping: a reappraisal. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[63]  P. Fraisse 6 – Rhythm and Tempo , 1982 .